Investigating Dyslexia through Diffusion Tensor Imaging across Ages: A Systematic Review. (2024)

Link/Page Citation

Author(s): Bruce Martins [1]; Mariana Yumi Baba [1]; Elisa Monteiro Dimateo [1]; Leticia Fruchi Costa [2]; Aila Silveira Camara [2]; Katerina Lukasova [2]; Mariana Penteado Nucci (corresponding author) [1,*]

1. Introduction

Dyslexia is a neurodevelopmental disorder that impairs fluency and/or speed of reading, as well as word recognition; it may also impact spelling. The difficulties should not be explained by other cognitive, health, or socio-economic factors [1]. The lack of accurate and fluent reading is a product of poor recognition and decoding abilities, and despite that, verbal comprehension is not equally affected in dyslexia [2,3].

Developmental dyslexia is classified within the framework of the diagnosis-specific learning disorder of reading [4], and it manifests in a spectrum from mild, to moderate, to severe [5]. It is present in over 80% of people with a learning disability in studied languages, having some variations regarding severity and type of difficulties according to the language structural characteristics [6]. Dyslexia’s mean prevalence is estimated at around 7% of the world population, with a predominance in male individuals [2,3]. Still, the incidence and prevalence are unclear due to the heterogeneity of literacy and language cultures with wide variances in terms of definitions, diagnostic instruments, rules, guidelines, and protocols for assessing dyslexic children and adults [1,7].

Two main medical classifications (ICD-11 and DSM-5) define the diagnostic criteria for dyslexia, and the assessment focuses mainly on identifying the reading and spelling discrepancies compared to the general population performance together with the assessment of other aspects that could confirm or rule out the diagnosis, for example, intelligence, phonological awareness, word and pseudoword reading, verbal fluency, verbal working memory, reading, and naming under stress [8]. This careful and time-consuming evaluation aims to support the clinical exclusion criteria, which contributes to reducing the prevalence by avoiding wrongful diagnoses of individuals struggling to read. Furthermore, there has been an effortful search in different fields for a better understanding of dyslexia’s development and neuroimaging contribution in clarifying neuroanatomical forming behind reading and dyslexia.

Clinical neuroimaging has been transformed by Diffusion-Weighted Imaging (DWI) and Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI), making it possible to examine the brain’s architecture and identify pathology earlier and more accurately than traditional magnetic resonance imaging sequences. Nowadays, diffusion is already used in clinical practice for stroke, trauma, tumors, demyelinating conditions, and neurosurgical planning. In psychiatric and neurological conditions, it is still used mainly in the research context [9].

The foundation of diffusion imaging lies in the behavior of water molecules, which move freely through space equally in all directions when unimpeded by structures. However, when encountering obstacles like cell membranes, water molecules tend to diffuse in alignment with the orientation of those barriers [10]. Magnetic resonance imaging, facilitated by DWI sequences, enables the measurement of water displacement in various directions for a brief duration. This information can then be used to assess tissue integrity, particularly in white matter fiber pathways [9].

Recent advancements in DTI research have expanded the focus beyond the assessment of a straightforward diffusion scalar to emphasize the significance of the more intricate 3D diffusion pattern. Axial diffusivity (AD), radial diffusivity (RD), mean diffusivity (MD), apparent diffusion coefficient (ADC), and others are additional imaging metrics that are increasingly used [11].

A standard DTI metric utilized in evaluating a variety of neuropathologic processes, from traumatic brain injury to demyelinating illness, is fractional anisotropy (FA), which quantifies the degree of this directionality [11]. Despite its promise for identifying subtle illnesses and alterations not discernible with conventional MRI sequences, the clinical applications of DTI have been questioned regarding the specificity of the findings [11].

In developmental neuroscience, DTI metrics have been shown to be a useful biomarker of white matter tract development and tissue injury, being used for treatment monitoring and potentially acting as outcome predictors. Children’s normal and pathological brain maturation has been described by the ADC/FA scalars since it is well known that as brain myelination and maturation advance, ADC values fall and FA values rise [12].

The literature has shown that people with dyslexia or reading disability may show brain structural changes with lower FA values in the left frontal and temporoparietal regions that coincide with the majority of studies on the left arcuate fasciculus (AF) and corona radiata (CR). Few studies have suggested a role for the posterior part of the corpus callosum or more ventral tracts like the inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF) or the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF) [13]. And more recently, a meta-analysis found no differences between dyslexics and typical readers when observing studies that conducted Voxel-Based Analysis (VBA) of FA and compared it to reading ability [14].

The purpose of this systematic review is to search for a better understanding of the multitude of possible methods for analyzing DTI data while accounting for the characteristics of a clinical population such as developmental dyslexia in the literature in the last 10 years.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Search Strategy

The systematic review searched the primary databases, PubMed and Scopus, for publications published within the last ten years, including the period from January 2011 to September 2022. The indexed articles were selected, and their findings were reported, following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines [15], and this study was not registered on Prospero. The criteria of interest selected were keywords in the following sequence: ((Dyslexia) AND (Brain connectivity) OR (Diffusion tensor imaging), using the boolean operators (DecS/MeSH):

SCOPUS: ((TITLE-ABS-KEY (dyslexia) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Reading disorder”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Reading disorders”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Reading disability”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Reading disabilities” [) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Developmental reading disability”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Developmental reading disabilities”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Developmental reading disorder”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Developmental reading disorders”))) AND ((TITLE-ABS-KEY (dti) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Diffusion Tensor MRI”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Diffusion Tensor Magnetic Resonance Imaging”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (tractography) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Diffusion Tractography”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Diffusion Tensor Imaging”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Diffusion weight imaging”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (dwi))) AND (LIMIT-TO (DOCTYPE, “ar”)) AND (LIMIT-TO (LANGUAGE, “English”)) AND (LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2022) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2021) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2020) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2019) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2018) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2017) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2016) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2015) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2014) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2013) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2012) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2011)).

PubMed: (((((((((Dyslexia*[Title/Abstract]) OR “Reading disorder”[Title/Abstract]) OR “Reading disorders”[Title/Abstract]) OR “Reading disability”[Title/Abstract]) OR “Reading disabilities”[Title/Abstract]) OR “Developmental reading disability”[Title/Abstract]) OR “Developmental reading disabilities”[Title/Abstract]) OR “Developmental reading disorder”[Title/Abstract]) OR “Developmental reading disorders”[Title/Abstract]) AND ((((((DTI[Title/Abstract]) OR “Diffusion Tensor MRI”[Title/Abstract]) OR “Diffusion Tensor Magnetic Resonance Imaging”[Title/Abstract]) OR “Diffusion Tractography”[Title/Abstract]) OR “Diffusion Tensor Imaging”[Title/Abstract]) OR Tractography[Title/Abstract])).

2.2. Inclusion Criteria

The review included only original articles written in English published within the last 10 years, and full text available about dyslexia in structural analyses by DTI. According to the patient/problem, intervention, comparison, and outcome (PICO) criterion, the problem was: the structural brain changes in dyslexia are unclear; the intervention was: structural analysis by DTI; the comparison was: differences between dyslexia and typical readers volunteers; and the outcome was: the structural brain pattern of dyslexia of development.

2.3. Exclusion Criteria

We excluded studies based on the following criteria: (i) reviews or meta-analyses; (ii) publications written in languages other than English; (iii) indexed articles published in more than one database (duplicates); (iv) articles that included dyslexia with other neurological or psychiatric comorbidities such as stroke, brain injury, epilepsy, autism, traumatic brain injury, aphasia, and mood disorders; (v) articles that performed any analysis other than DTI, such as machine learning, graph theory, and only methodological comparison; (vi) articles in which the diagnosis of dyslexia is unclear; (vii) articles without at least one outcome or method of analysis reporting DTI measures and/or correlation with demographic or neuropsychological measures; (viii) case reports; (ix) neonates; and (x) dyslexia with genetic alterations.

2.4. Data Compilation

In this review, seven of the authors (B.M., M.Y.B., E.M.D., L.F.C., A.S.C., K.L., and M.P.N.), in pairs, independently and randomly analyzed, reviewed, and assessed the eligibility of titles and abstracts according to the strategy of established search. The authors B.M., M.Y.B., E.M.D., L.F.C., A.S.C., K.L., and M.P.N. selected the final articles by evaluating the texts that met the selection criteria. The authors B.M., E.M.D., L.F.C., and A.S.C. were responsible for the search for the demographic, clinical, and neuropsychological characteristics of volunteers and dyslexia patients, being checked by the senior authors (K.L. and M.P.N.). The authors B.M., M.Y.B., L.F.C., and A.S.C. searched for the characteristics of structural brain analyses and their outcomes, and all data were checked by the senior authors (K.L. and M.P.N.). All of the authors contributed to writing the entire text of this review.

2.5. Data Extraction

The selected articles were analyzed using two topics, which were represented in tables that addressed the following characteristics: (1) the demographic characteristics of the population sample, their language, their nationality, and the neuropsychological tools used to characterize the reading disorder; and (2) the characteristics of DTI acquisition, the parameters used in the image analyses and corrections, the structural outcomes between groups, and the correlation with clinical data when reported.

2.6. Risk of Bias Assessment

The selection of articles was performed in pairs, and a third independent author decided if the articles should be included. The data selected in the tables were divided by the authors into the groups already described above, and the checking of the data was carried out by the following group. The final inclusion of studies into the systematic review was by agreement of all reviewers.

2.7. Data Analysis

The data from the articles included in the tables were analyzed descriptively using the percentage, mean, and standard deviation; the variation to characterize each factor attributed to the demographic and neuropsychological characteristics of the participants in each study; and the characteristics of the acquisition, analysis, and results of the structural assessment performed by DTI image acquisition.

3. Results

3.1. Overview of the Screening Process of the Included Studies

Following the inclusion and exclusion criteria described above, we found 124 articles in the last ten years throughout the Scopus and PubMed databases, with 116 from Scopus and 8 from PubMed. Of the 116 articles found in Scopus, 64 were excluded after screening, two studies were with participants with alexia, one with aphasia, two with autism, one with mood disorder, two neonates, 20 without dyslexia diagnosis, two with genetic alterations, nine with brain injury (such as stroke, epilepsy, cortical lesion, and TBI), six were case reports, three were meta-analyses, seven were reviews, three were methodological studies, and six only featured morphometric analyses. The eligibility analysis excluded a further four articles. Three studies reported different methodologies of DTI analysis (machine learning, graph theory, and manual and automatic segmentation), and one did not report the DTI results, resulting in 48 studies included in this selection. Out of the eight articles identified in PubMed, five were excluded during screening: two lacked a dyslexia diagnosis, and three were duplicates from Scopus. After the eligibility assessment, an additional two studies were excluded. One study conducted DTI analysis using machine learning, while another did not report DTI results. Consequently, only one study from PubMed was included [16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59] in this systematic review, 48 were included from Scopus, and one was included from PubMed, as shown in Figure 1.

3.2. Demographic and Neuropsychological Characteristics of Studied Dyslexia Subjects

This systematic review aimed to provide an overview of dyslexia in various stages of life, from early childhood (preschool to kindergarten) with a familiar history of dyslexia to elementary school children, adolescents, and adults, following the structural brain changes involved in this neurobiological disorder. In the selected studies about dyslexia and structural brain analysis by tractography included in this systematic review, 15% of studies [16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23] included only children below 6 years old with the family risk of dyslexia before reading acquisition (classified as pre-readers); followed by 70% of the studies that analyzed older groups, at different stages of reading proficiency (classified as reading children) from 7 years old up to 19, males and females (classified as readers); and 15% of studied male and female adults aged from 20 to 33 years old (classified as reading adults) (Table 1).

Another important aspect is that dyslexia can manifest similarly across languages, but the characteristics and challenges may vary based on the language’s structure, writing system, and phonological rules. Because of this, we include the demographic characteristics of the country and the language participants spoke. The pre-reader studies were done mainly in the United States with English language speakers (50%) [16,17,18,22], followed by 40% [20,21,23] in Belgium with Dutch speakers, and 10% [19] in Germany with German speakers, including young children under 6 years old male and females and balanced distribution (a total of 193 females to 215 males) and with a sample variation of 10 to 46 children in each comparison group. In studies with reading-stage children, 47% spoke English—from the USA (45%) [25,27,31,32,33,39,42,44,48,50,52,53,54,56] and Canada (2%) [28,29]; 17% spoke French (the study was carried out in France [26,30,34,38,45]); 11% spoke Dutch—the studies were carried out in Belgium and Netherlands [37,41]; 8% were German; 6% spoke Mandarin—the studies were carried out in China [43] and Taiwan [36]; and 3% of studies were done with speakers of Arabic (Egypt) [35], Spanish (from Spain) [47], Portuguese (from Brazil) [49], or Italian (from Italy) [51]. In adults, the language of studies was less varied: 38% spoke English, and the studies were carried out in New Zealand [59] and the USA [62,64]; 25% spoke German (Germany) [58,60], 25% Dutch (Belgium) [61,63], and 12% Finnish (Finland) [57] (Table 1).

The neuropsychological characterization of the studied subjects followed the specificity of dyslexia diagnostic criteria, that the neuropsychological tests that were used included assessment of intelligence quotient (IQ); word and non-word reading and spelling tests; reading comprehension tests; rapid automatized naming (RAN); phonological awareness; and language, attention, and executive functions (description in Table 1). The primary purpose of the neuropsychological evaluation was to compare the performance of the dyslexic groups with the control group.

In the pre-reader, the intelligence was evaluated only by non-verbal tests, and only one study [16] showed a significant difference, with a worse performance of a risk of dyslexic children compared to the typically developing ones. One study did not report an intelligence assessment, possibly due to the very early age of participants [18]. In the reader children group, almost all studies evaluated the IQ (94.6%) with 51.4% by non-verbal and verbal IQ tests [25,26,27,30,31,38,39,40,45,47,48,49,52,53,54], 40.5% with only non-verbal tests [25,28,33,34,36,41,44,46,55,56], and 2.7% with only the verbal tests [50]. The significant difference between groups occurred in 34.3% of studies in verbal IQ tests and only 5.9% in non-verbal IQ tests. In the adult readers, 57.1% of the studies were administered non-verbal IQ tests [57,60,61,63,64] and only 28.6% showed significant differences with worse outcomes for dyslexic adults.

Concerning reading skills, in pre-readers, 89% [16,17,19,20,21,22,23] of studies assessed the children by word, letters, or pseudoword reading, and out of these 44% [16,17,19,21,22,23] had significantly lower outcome compared to the control group. In this age range, the RAN was also generally employed (78%) [16,17,19,20,22,23], with objects and colors being the most commonly used items (56%). A significant difference was found in naming speeds, mainly for slower naming of objects by dyslexic children in 44% of studies of this population. The phonological awareness was also mostly assessed (75%) [16,17,19,20,21,22,23], and in 25% of studies performance was lower in dyslexic groups. As for the other cognitive functions, attention was assessed in just one study [19], and the findings on working memory, digit span, visual reception, and gross and fine motor were also seldom reported.

Regarding the reader children, in all of the studies the reading skills were assessed by the word reading, and as would be expected lower performance of children with dyslexia compared to controls was found in 79% of studies [24,27,30,31,32,33,34,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56]. Other significant between-group differences with worse outcomes for dyslexic children were reported for pseudoword reading (70% [24,27,28,30,31,32,33,34,38,39,40,41,44,45,46,47,48,51,52,53,54,55,56] out of 85% assessed studies) and text reading (39% [24,30,32,34,39,40,45,46,51,52,54,55,56] out of 64% assessed studies). In 45% of all studies, the phonological awareness was assessed [24,26,27,31,34,35,38,39,40,41,43,45,46,51,53], and 39% of studies [24,26,27,31,34,38,39,40,41,43,45,46,51,53] produced significant results when compared between groups. When assessing RAN, 39% of studies evaluated this ability [24,26,29,34,35,38,40,41,43,45,46,52,56], and 33% found a significantly worse outcome for the dyslexic children when compared between groups [24,26,34,38,40,41,43,45,46,52,56]. A small number of studies (9%) assessed language [38,43,56] and attention [24,27,40]; however, almost all of these studies showed worse performance of subjects with dyslexia when compared to the controls. Half of the studies reported results of other cognitive functions in different aspects of short-term memory (digit span 33.3%) [24,26,27,38,40,45,46], verbal working memory (33.3%) [24,26,38,40,43,46], working memory (17%) [41,47], and arithmetic or mood behavior (11%) [46,56]. Almost all of these assessments also reported significant between-group differences and lower outcomes for participants with dyslexia.

For the reading adults, 100% of studies assessed volunteers for the word and/or pseudoword reading [57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64]. In 37.5% of the studies [57,58,62,64], text reading was tested, and all tests produced significant findings that allowed the groups to be distinguished based on the lower performance of the dyslexic group. Only 50% of these studies that tested RAN and phonological awareness showed significant group differences [57,59,63,64], and 25% assessed language and attention domains [57,59], without significant group differences.

Considering the neuropsychological outcome of reading children in the three language groups most represented in this revision (English, French, and Dutch), there was no difference in the cognitive skills found impaired in dyslexic volunteers compared to the controls.

3.3. Brain Structural Connectivity Characteristics on Acquisition, Process, and Outcomes of Dyslexia

The structural analysis through the DTI acquisition was performed in 88% of studies in high-field MRI equipment (3T) (scanner by manufacturers: Siemens (Berlin, Germany) (49%) [16,17,18,19,22,24,25,26,30,31,32,34,36,38,40,41,43,44,45,46,55,57,58,60], Philips (Eindhoven, Netherlands) (35%) [20,21,23,27,33,37,47,48,50,51,52,53,54,61,63,64], General Electric (GE, New York, United States of America) (4%)) [28,56] and 10% in low-field (1.5T) (scanner by manufacturers: Siemens (6%) [39,59,62], Philips or GE (2%) [35,49]) used in the acquisition of older participants such as reader children (more than 7 year old) and adults (more than 18 year old), as shown in Table 2.

Most of the selected studies (91.8%) used the sequence DTI for the diffusion analysis, and only 6.1% used diffusion kurtosis imaging (DKI) protocols [25,32,33] that require at least 3 b-values (as compared to 2 b-values for DTI) and at least 30 independent diffusion gradient directions (as compared to 6 for DTI), in which these 3 b-values were used: 0, 700 or 800 or 1000, and 2000 s/mm[sup.2] with 30 or 32, and 64 noncollinear diffusion directions. In two studies, the DTI sequence also was reported with 3 b-values (0, 700, or 1000, and 1000 or 2000 for b-values with 64 diffusion directions), 50% of DTI studies used 2 b-values (0 and 700 or 800 or 1000 or 1400) and 37% studies only one b-value (700 or 800 or 1000 or 1300 or 1400 or 5000). Regarding the number of noncollinear diffusion gradient directions in the studies that acquired DTI sequence, 41.3% reported more than 60 directions (28.3% was 60 and 2.2% was 128 directions), 34.8% used between 30 to 56 directions, 13% of studies used less than 30 directions (the smaller number of directions was 6), and 10.9% did not report this parameter [46] (Table 2).

The basic pulse sequence repetition time (TR) and echo time (TE) parameters ranged from 3000 to 14,000 ms and from 55 to 110 ms, respectively; the slice image ranged from 23 slices with 5 mm of thickness to 160 slices with around 1.7 mm to cover the entire brain, and the field of view (FOV) ranged from 208 to 282 mm.

The DTI analysis was performed in the selected studies by different softwares, and usually (82%) used more than one software to conduct all of the analysis. Most of the studies (63%) used the FSL software [18,19,23,24,25,26,28,30,31,32,33,38,39,40,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,52,53,55,58,59,60,61,63] and its different tools (Neurite orientation dispersion and the density imaging (NODDI) model, Tract-Based Spatial Statistics (TBSS), FDT, DTIFIT, PR0OBTRACKX, and BEDPOSTX, among others) associated or not with other software; 18% of studies [16,17,18,21,22,27,32,40,41] used the VistaLab developed at Stanford University that comprises different tools such as MrDiffusion and Automated Fiber Quantification (AFQ); 20% used ExploreDTI [20,23,24,29,37,38,43,45,61,63]; 16% used the TrackVis [18,20,23,24,26,37,43,45]; 12% DTIprep [16,17,18,22,44,50], 10% MRTrix [19,25,32,40,57]; 10% SPM [41,51,56,62,64]; 6% TRActs Constrained by UnderLying Anatomy (TRACULA) [33,44,52]; 4% DSI Studio [36,57]; and 2% BrainVoyager [34,51], the only commercial software; and few studies used PANDA, DIPY, Reproducible Objective Quantification Scheme (ROQS), TORTOISE, and TractSeg.

Artifacts in DWI acquisitions lead to errors in tensor estimation, and Eddy Current (EC) distortions and Head Motion (HM) are the two primary intrinsic DTI acquisition abnormalities that may obliterate the voxel-wise correlation across all the DWIs. Most of the selected studies (73%) reported in the pre-processing step comprise the EC and HM (with a cutoff from 1.5 mm to 6 mm) corrections. Other studies (10%) reported corrections to the image (EPI) distortions, 4% for the Gibbs artifact (truncation or ringing artifact) or Marchenko–Pastur Principal Component Analysis (MP-PCA), and only 14% did not report any correction in the preprocessing image step.

Some FSL tools were used for these corrections such as CATNAP (Coregistration, Adjustment, and Tensor-solving a Nicely Automated Program), which is a data processing pipeline for Philips PAR/REC Magnetic Resonance data files, performing motion correction for both diffusion and structural images using FSL FLIRT; it adjusts the diffusion gradient directions for scanner settings (i.e., slice angulation, slice orientation, etc.) and motion correction (i.e., the rotational component of the applied transformation) and computes tensor and derived quantities (FA, MD, colormaps, eigenvalues, etc). Also, the TOPUP tool of FSL is used to correct images of the susceptibility-induced distortions (fix EPI distortions), and QUAD (Quality Assessment for DMRI) for automatically performing image quality control (QC) at the single subject.

The tracking of DTI group analysis was normally reported as whole brain, tract, or ROI-based, from voxel-based, and 34.7% of studies reported as ROI-based methods [17,18,20,21,22,34,35,37,39,42,48,50,52,53,54,60,63], 26.5% as whole brain and ROI-based methods [16,19,23,27,28,29,30,31,33,41,43,45,47], 22.4% only whole brain or fiber tract-based analysis [24,25,26,32,36,38,40,44,46,57,64], and 12.2% as voxel-based analysis [49,51,55,58,59,62].

Regarding DTI quantitative analysis, 92% measured FA; of these, 29% also reported AD or RD [18,22,31,39,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,54,61], and 22% MD. In a few studies, other anisotropy metrics were used such as 4% relative anisotropy (RA) [42,50], 2% QA [57], or hindrance-modulated orientation anisotropy (HMOA) [45]. The MD measure was also described by directionally averaged mean diffusivity (Dav) [54,64] and extra-axonal mean diffusivity (MDe) [32], in 4% and 2% of studies, respectively. The ADC [35] or exponential apparent diffusion coefficient (eADC) metrics were reported in 2% of studies. Some White Matter Tract Integrity (WMTI) metrics from DKI were reported in 2% of studies, such as axonal water fraction (AWF) [32], intra-axonal diffusivity (Da) [32], or MDe [32], and NODDI metrics such as Neurite density index (NDI) and Orientation dispersion index (ODI) were reported in 4% of studies each [31,33].

Different types of atlas were reported in the selected studies to segment the cortical and white matter region; 54% used one of FSL atlas such as Julich-Brain Cytoarchitectonic Atlas, MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) Structural Atlas, JHU (Johns Hopkins University) ICBM-DTI-81 White-Matter Tractography Atlas, and Harvard-Oxford atlas; 19% used one of three FS’s atlas (Desikan–Killiany, Destrieux, and Desikan–Killiany–Tourville cortical atlas); 9% used native space; 7% used the automated anatomical atlas (AAL), the template for SPM, AFQ, ExploreDTI, and PANDA software; 3% used Talairach atlas; and 9% did not report this information.

Most of the studies (90%) specified the tracts/ROI used to explore the group comparison or association between structural data and demographic or neuropsychological data, the main tracts were AF (49%) [16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,25,27,32,34,35,37,40,41,43,44,45,47,51,59,61,63], inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF) (41%) [17,22,25,27,32,34,37,39,40,41,42,43,44,46,48,49,50,51,53,62], inferior frontal-occipital fasciculus (IFOF) (39%) [20,23,25,32,34,36,37,39,40,41,43,45,47,49,50,51,62,63,64], superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF) (37%) [16,17,22,25,27,31,32,35,39,40,41,42,44,45,47,50,51,56], 24% for corpus callosum (CC) [17,25,36,39,47,51,52,53,54,55,61,62], 18% for corticospinal (CS) tract [18,32,35,39,40,41,42,49,50], 12% for CR (including anterior and posterior parts—aCR and pCR) [35,39,55,59,62,64], 20% for uncinate fasciculus (UF) [24,25,32,37,39,40,41,43,49,50], and 16% for thalamic radiation (ThR) (including anterior and posterior parts- aThR and pThR) [32,36,40,41,49,50,52,62]. Forceps minor (FMn) and major were reported in 10% of studies [32,40,41,49,50], as well as the cingulum (CG) [31,40,42,49,50]; less frequently was also reported in 4% optical radiation (OR) [42,51], as well as cerebellar peduncles [36], internal capsule [62], temporal and temporo-occipital regions [19], frontal aslant tract (FAT) [24], primary auditory cortex, lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) [60], and inferior colliculus (2% each). 6% of studies reported only the atlas used [26,29,30], did not specify the tracts or ROIs, and 4% did not report this information [46,57].

Regarding the results of the structural analysis of the brain, 89% of the studies described this finding, with the predominance of a decrease in FA in the left hemisphere, when comparing the dyslexia group with the control group, and 10% of the studies did not show a significant difference between the groups [19,25,46,59,64], occurring mainly in the group of adults (25%) [59,64].

The tractography analyses of the pre-reader group reported by the selected studies were based on the FA changes according to the risk of familial history of dyslexia (FHD); 13% reported higher FA of right SLF [16] or CC [17] compared to the children with positive FHD than TR negative FHD, and 50% showed lower FA of the left AF (as also long portion) [18,21,22,23] and 13% in IFOF [20], as shown in Figure 2, highlighting the frequency in green color. Almost all of these regions also showed a positive correlation with age and some language tests and predicted decoding skills or reading impairment.

In the reading children, 42% of these studies reported a significant change in FA values (27% lower [27,38,42,43,44,45,47,49,55] and 15% higher values [40,41,48,52,54] in dyslexic children than the TR group), decreasing mainly in the left tracts such as AF, SLF, ILF, CR, and IFOF and increasing in the right SLF, aThR, cingulum, and CC. These studies also showed 6% of AD [43,50] decreased in dyslexic children in IFOF, SLF, UF, FMn, CG, and the right ThR [50], as well as in the left AF and ILF [43], and 12% of MD [28,32,39] changes between groups (6% higher [28,32] and 6% lower values in dyslexic children [32,39]). Only 3% of these studies reported higher values of ODI in the left ventral optical tract [33] of DYX and lower values of RD and HMOA (in UF of DYX males) [24], as shown in Figure 2, highlighting the frequency in red color. No group differences were reported in 6% of these studies [25,46], and 30% did not report brain changes [26,29,30,31,34,35,36,37,51,56]

In the selected studies with adult participants, 50% showed group differences, in which the DYX had low FA in lateral geniculate nucleus, TOC, and left-AF [60,63], and another DTI metric was the QA with high values for DYX group in left- UF, CS, CC, ThR, FMj, and parietal tracts and low in left SLF, CS, and OF in comparison to TR group [57], as shown in Figure 2, highlighting the frequency in purple color. In total, 25% of these studies did not find a group difference [59,64] or report this information [61,62].

The outcomes were also reported by the correlation analysis between clinical, demographic, and neuropsychological data, with white matter changes in 83.6% of studies, in which 60.9% showed a positive correlation between FA/MD changes and age (12%) [18,25,39,41,48,54]; gender (4%) [25,29], mainly in children; and neuropsychological tests (phonological awareness, word/no-word identification skill, VAS, working memory, verbal fluency, digit span, and Chinese character recognition) (44.9%) [20,22,24,25,27,29,30,31,32,35,36,37,38,39,43,44,46,54,56,57,61,63] in all ages; 32.6% reported a negative correlation between FA/MD/RD changes and neuropsychological performance [28,30,32,34,35,37,39,40,43,44,45,46,52,57,60,61]; 5% of studies reported [19,21,23] changes between white matter and DYX diagnoses or neuropsychological outcomes/skills; and 16.4% did not find or report correlation results [33,42,47,49,50,53,55,59].

4. Discussion

This systematic review provided an overview of the main structural brain changes findings by the DTI technique in developmental dyslexia in different age groups, including early at-risk children due to a family history of the disorder, as well as children and adults with reading disorders who have been diagnosed with dyslexia. By comparing the results of several studies, we describe converging evidence on structural abnormalities of the brain and the associations between imaging results and changes in this population’s characteristics. This systematic review observed that the assessment of different cognitive functions by the neuropsychological instruments of the selected studies varied according to age, country, and language; however, the lack of standardized procedures among age and language groups drastically reduces the possibility of comparing the behavioral outcomes and its relation to the structural brain changes. While some aspects of reading, mainly word decoding and recognition, are the most assessed skills in all the age groups, others such as language, attention, and working memory are reported exceptionally. The intelligence measure showed an expected outcome in terms of experimental and control group matching, with more studies reporting lower performance of children with dyslexia on verbal but not on non-verbal tests of intelligence. The intelligence assessment has generated a long-time debate in the field of learning, and while in typically developing children intelligence performance generally correlates with the achievement level, in reading impairment the intellectual disability is inconsistent with a diagnosis of dyslexia [65].

The importance of matching groups on intelligence measures is also supported by a growing body of evidence linking the brain white matter organization to intelligence level and processing efficiency. A recent study showed stronger integration of white matter structures within a local community (ex. frontal region) and especially with external brain networks (ex. frontal to parietal regions) in adults scoring high on non-verbal tests compared to average performers [66].

The structural brain changes shown by the DTI measurements were examined or extracted using the atlases, and nearly all studies (92%) examined the fractional anisotropy, which represents the directionality and organization of tissue microstructure; other studies examined some of its variants, including RA, QA, and HMOA, providing additional information to FA. The studies also examined the following measures: the AD measure, which can show changes in the density or integrity of axons within white matter pathways; the RD measure, whose increase frequently denotes disruptions in the microstructure of white matter, such as demyelination or axonal damage; and the MD measure, which, along with some variations like Dav and MDe, typically shows higher values that indicate decreased tissue integrity and increased diffusion.

In children before reading acquisition who had a positive family history of dyslexia, the structural changes analyzed by DTI metrics were shown mainly by the FA alterations. The decrease in FA was predominant in the left of the AF, as well as for IFOF, a result also reported in a recent study with dyslexic children with this profile [21], and a pattern of increased FA occurred in few studies, and only in the right hemisphere of the SLF and sCC, which may suggest possible early neural compensatory mechanisms in the right hemisphere [17]. This pattern was also similar in reading children, with low FA values mainly in the left hemisphere involving the AF, but also high FA values in the right hemisphere of AF, showing more neuroplasticity signals than the other young group. In addition, in these reader groups the structural changes covered more areas, still with a predominance of the left hemisphere, and were identified in other DTI metrics, such as low AD values in AF, SLF, IFOF, UF, ThR, ILF, CG, and CS, as well as a high MD values in AF, and low in UF and CR. Children submitted to reading intervention show an increase in MD values in AF and other reading brain circuitry such as the left ILF and posterior CC [67]. Variations of anisotropy, such as RA and HMOA, also showed low values in brain structural changes reported in the articles with reader children, but only FA showed high values in the following tracts AF, SLF, CC, ThR, ILF, and CG.

In dyslexic adults, the percentage of structural changes reported was much lower than those reported in children. It seems that with brain development, in adulthood, the structural differences of dyslexia become less evident due to neuroadaptation. However, the pattern of decreased FA measurements in dyslexic adults remained the same, with predominance in the left hemisphere of AF and the region that comprises the AF (lateral geniculate nucleus, and temporo-occipital cortex), as well as bilaterally SLF, CC, CS, and the left of UF and ThR by the QA measurements. This anisotropy variation, the QA measure, also showed an increase in the left side of SLF, VOF, and CS, which may represent neuroplasticity in adulthood. Nonetheless, due to literature scarcity on dyslexia in adults it is difficult to compare these results found in the review with other studies.

Another form of result widely explored between studies was the analysis of association, be it through correlation or the prediction of structural data with demographic or neuropsychological data, and this occurred with a greater incidence of significant findings between studies than the actual comparison of structural changes. These association analyses are normally applied to assist in the interpretation of results, mainly in studies with adults when there were no structural difference between the groups, but measures mainly of FA were positively or negatively correlated with the results of neuropsychological tests. In children, the structural changes helped predict some performance in neuropsychological tests, especially in children with a family risk of dyslexia.

In addition to the structural results found in participants with dyslexia and controls, this review considered how DTI data were acquired, processed, and extracted as an analysis. In general, all studies included in this review took good care to ensure good image and data quality, reducing bias in the interpretation of results.

The DTI acquired in high magnetic field equipment, such as 3 Tesla, can increase the capacity to acquire higher resolution scans more quickly, with higher b-values and thinner slices, as well as to increase tissue contrast and reduce background noise (thereby increasing the signal-to-noise ratio and contrast-to-noise ratio) [68]; this magnetic field was used in 88% of the studies (49% Siemens, 35% Philips, and 4% GE), in the acquisition of both DKI and DTI sequences to study tractography. The latter sequence is the more traditional sequence and was used in most of the selected articles in our systematic review. The difference between the two is that DKI significantly reduces the error of dODF orientation estimates compared to DTI and makes it possible to detect crossing fibers, which leads to a noticeable improvement in tractography across regions with complex fiber bundle geometries [69,70]. DKI-based tractography has potential benefits, especially in clinical contexts when time is of the essence [71].

The acquisition parameters of the DTI is important because they affect values of white matter (WM) scalar metrics, including FA, MD, Signal Noise Ratio (SNR), and even entire brain tractography investigations. These acquisition parameters include the diffusion sensitivity coefficient (b-value), which is a factor that reflects the strength and timing of those gradients used to generate DWI, as well as the reliability of DTI results concerning image and data quality; diffusion directions, in which there are more directions the longer the acquisition time; and voxel size (the smaller the size, the higher the quality [72,73]).

The adequate b-value for diffusion imaging quality evaluation in dyslexia was unclear and varied according to the equipment’s magnetic field. In a heath brain, a greater b-value usually results in a poorer SNR and image quality because increased signal attenuation owing to diffusion as well as increased TE (and therefore additional signal loss due to T2 decay), would lead to the decrease of MD, AD, and RD when the gradient directions and voxel resolution remained constant [73,74]. In the literature, this is more evident at the low field strength of the MR scanner as 1.5 T [75], and only 10% of the selected studies of this review used this field, less evident in high field strength (3 T and 7 T) due to their relatively high SNR, in which the increase in b value has little influence on the decrease in SNR, showing the best image and data quality with the respective b-values, 200 and 900 s/mm[sup.2] [73]. The selected studies included in this review reported normally more than one b-value and the MRI of 1.5T used b-values ranging from 800 to 1000 s/mm[sup.2] and the 3 T from 700 to 5000 s/mm[sup.2] [73,74].

The increased number of diffusion-encoding gradient directions can also improve DTI quality by averaging and strengthening the tensor estimation, and the opposite can reduce all DTI scalar values’ accuracy and precision; a minimum of 18 diffusion directions is advised to produce trustworthy DTI scalar results using the TBSS toolbox of FSL software [76]. In our study, just a small number of the selected studies (11.8%) used fewer than 30 directions, and of these, 7.8% used less than 18 directions, whereas the majority (78.4%) used 30 directions or more (41.3% used more than 60 directions).

Considering the impact of MRI acquisition parameters on the values of DTI measures, changes in the number of gradients and voxel resolution have the greatest impact on the FA, but variations in the b-value have a special effect on MD [72]. Another study also showed that the number of gradient directions was more relevant than the spatial resolution in some quantitative measures of DTI, such as tract volume, median fiber density, and mean FA, but this did not occur for all tracts evaluated in the same way, only for SLF and IFOF [77].

One of the most defining and defiant components of building a tracking algorithm is determining the underlying model that connects the raw dMRI images to the local fiber orientations, and presently, there is a wide range of software packages that incorporate higher-order fiber-tracking techniques that can calculate the relative contributions and orientations of several fiber populations within each voxel, which are easily applied to clinically relevant data sets [78].

A wide range of processing functions are offered by these software packages, such as tensor calculations, fiber tracking, visualization, statistical analysis, quantitative measure extraction from DTI datasets, and integration with additional neuroimaging tools. They vary, though, when it comes to the tractography algorithms that are applied, such as probabilistic, which produces a vast collection or distribution of potential trajectories from each seed point, and deterministic, which assumes a unique fiber orientation estimate in each voxel [79]; as well as the local approach, which is a fast and widely used method, it follows the local orientations of previously extracted fibers independently of each other, but the sum of small errors in these local orientations can significantly affect the final result, making it a very weak predictor of data with little quantitative significance or biological [78]. On the other hand, the global methods offer improved stability concerning noise and imaging artifacts and a greater agreement with the real dMRI data that was recorded; however, they rely on stochastic optimization approaches and hence do not guarantee convergence to a globally optimal solution [80].

The choice of software to analyze the DTI normally depends on the specific analysis requirements, familiarity with the software, and preference for user interface and workflow, and the studies normally used more than one software to employ specific tools to conduct the entire analysis.

Several image adjustments are usually conducted before DTI analysis to enhance the data quality and reduce common artifacts that may occur during data acquisition, allowing for appropriate interpretation and trustworthy outcomes. Most of the adjustments reported by the studies were Eddy current correction (74%) which aligns the DWI to a reference image acquired without diffusion weighting [81], and motion correction (74%); few explore structural analyses but they realign the image to compensate for subject motion, ensuring that the diffusion measurements are accurate and consistent across the dataset [82]. However, in low frequency the Gibbs ringing correction (4%) of the discontinuities in the k-space data caused by undersampling during image acquisition was also reported, resulting in obscure anatomical structures and affecting diffusion measurements, as well as EPI distortion correction (10%), considering the spatial variations in the strength and direction of the magnetic field gradients used for diffusion encoding. Unhappily, 14% of the studies did not report any adjustment before DTI analyses.

Regarding the type of quantitative DTI analyses reported by the studies, most (69.2%) used voxel-wise, which analyzes each voxel to identify differences in diffusion properties between groups or conditions, providing more detailed spatial information about diffusion metrics at a voxel-level; this is present in some of the software such as FSL, SPM, MRItrix, and ExploreDTI. Interestingly, a lot of articles reported this analysis as the whole brain since voxel-based analysis includes all the cerebro voxels. Also, ROI-based analysis used in 61.5% of studies is frequently conducted after voxel-based, making it challenging to determine which method was used in each study.

The outcome description of the studies was based on anatomical regions of interest or specific tracts outlined by a well-established atlas that facilitates the interpretation of imaging data by providing standardized anatomical labeling and spatial coordinates. While the atlases share the goal of delineating brain structures and regions, they differ in several aspects, including their origins, resolutions, and intended applications.

The study’s limitations stem mainly from the differences in how dyslexia was defined across studies. The use of the terms dyslexia, developmental dyslexia, reading disorders, or never reading difficulties point to the lack of a common terminology and diagnosis criteria. The impact of the findings of this study could be that the results may be possibly drawn from a very heterogeneous sample. However, the cognitive testing of participants may ameliorate this and emerge as a potential tool mainly in internationally normed tests.

Another limitation of the study was the influence of language on dyslexia’s reading acquisition history. It is a well-known fact that readers in irregular language systems have longer reading acquisition and struggling readers may develop different compensation strategies.

Studies on neuroimaging may be limited by variations in image acquisition parameterization; however, despite this wide range, all studies used optimal acquisition and analysis parameters that did not affect the comparability of results, even in cases where certain fundamental information was not stated. In terms of results, a certain study focused on describing the anatomical regions examined rather than the tract as most studies did, taking into account the tracts involved in the regions described.

5. Conclusions

This systematic review of structural alterations in the brain associated with dyslexia revealed that over the past ten years studies on children have outnumbered those on adults, primarily focusing on boys and the English language. The studies also showed that brain changes concentrated in FA reduction in the fasciculus arcuate of the left hemisphere at all ages, and in the left superior longitudinal fascicle for reading in children and adults, as well as an increase in the right hemisphere, which may indicate signs of neuroadaptation. A better understanding of structural brain changes of dyslexia and neuroadaptations can be a guide for future interventions.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization—B.M., K.L. and M.P.N.; methodology and validation—B.M., M.Y.B., E.M.D., L.F.C., K.L. and M.P.N.; investigation and formal analysis—B.M., M.Y.B., E.M.D., L.F.C. and A.S.C.; writing—-original draft preparation, B.M., M.Y.B., K.L. and M.P.N.; writing—review and editing, B.M., K.L. and M.P.N.; and supervision and project administration—M.P.N. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES)—Programa de Demanda Social (code 001).

References

1. American Psychiatric Association, 5th ed. edition; American Psychiatric Association Publishing: Washington, DC, USA, 2022, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425787.. Text Rev.

2. R.L. Peterson; B.F. Pennington Developmental dyslexia., 2012, 379,pp. 1997-2007. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60198-6.

3. R.L. Peterson; B.F. Pennington Developmental dyslexia., 2015, 11,pp. 283-307. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112842.

4. R.L. Peterson; L.M. McGrath; E.G. Willcutt; J.M. Keenan; R.K. Olson; B.F. Pennington How specific are learning disabilities?., 2021, 54,pp. 466-483. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219420982981.

5. M. Habib The neurological basis of developmental dyslexia and related disorders: A reappraisal of the temporal hypothesis, twenty years on., 2021, 11, 708. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11060708.

6. N.M. Majeed; A. Hartanto; J.J. Tan Developmental dyslexia and creativity: A meta-analysis., 2021, 27,pp. 187-203. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1677.

7. C. Di Folco; A. Guez; H. Peyre; F. Ramus Epidemiology of developmental dyslexia: A comparison of DSM-5 and ICD-11 criteria., 2020,pp. 1-34. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.12.18.20248189.

8. J. Roitsch; S.M. Watson An overview of dyslexia: Definition, characteristics, assessment, identification, and intervention., 2019, 7, DOI: https://doi.org/10.11648/j.sjedu.20190704.11.

9. W. Van Hecke; L. Emsell; S. Sunaert, Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2016, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-3118-7.

10. C. Beaulieu The basis of anisotropic water diffusion in the nervous system—A technical review., 2002, 15,pp. 435-455. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/nbm.782.

11. A.J. Steven; J. Zhuo; E.R. Melhem Diffusion kurtosis imaging: An emerging technique for evaluating the microstructural environment of the brain., 2014, 202,pp. W26-W33. DOI: https://doi.org/10.2214/AJR.13.11365.

12. T. Huisman Diffusion-weighted and diffusion tensor imaging of the brain, made easy., 2010, 10,p. S163. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1102/1470-7330.2010.9023.

13. M. Vandermosten; B. Boets; J. Wouters; P. Ghesquière A qualitative and quantitative review of diffusion tensor imaging studies in reading and dyslexia., 2012, 36,pp. 1532-1552. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.04.002.

14. D. Moreau; J.E. Stonyer; N.S. McKay; K.E. Waldie No evidence for systematic white matter correlates of dyslexia: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis., 2018, 1683,pp. 36-47. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2018.01.014.

15. M.J. Page; J.E. McKenzie; P.M. Bossuyt; I. Boutron; T.C. Hoffmann; C.D. Mulrow; L. Shamseer; J.M. Tetzlaff; E.A. Akl; S.E. Brennan et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews., 2021, 88,p. 105906. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijsu.2021.105906.

16. J. Zuk; J. Dunstan; E. Norton; X. Yu; O. Ozernov-Palchik; Y. Wang; T.P. Hogan; J.D. Gabrieli; N. Gaab Multifactorial pathways facilitate resilience among kindergarteners at risk for dyslexia: A longitudinal behavioral and neuroimaging study., 2021, 24,p. e12983. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12983.

17. X. Yu; J. Zuk; M.V. Perdue; O. Ozernov-Palchik; T. Raney; S.D. Beach; E.S. Norton; Y. Ou; J.D. Gabrieli; N. Gaab Putative protective neural mechanisms in prereaders with a family history of dyslexia who subsequently develop typical reading skills., 2020, 41,pp. 2827-2845. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24980.

18. N. Langer; B. Peysakhovich; J. Zuk; M. Drottar; D.D. Sliva; S. Smith; B.L. Becker; P.E. Grant; N. Gaab White matter alterations in infants at risk for developmental dyslexia., 2017, 27,pp. 1027-1036. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv281.

19. I. Kraft; J. Schreiber; R. Cafiero; R. Metere; G. Schaadt; J. Brauer; N.E. Neef; B. Müller; H. Kirsten; A. Wilcke et al. Predicting early signs of dyslexia at a preliterate age by combining behavioral assessment with structural MRI., 2016, 143,pp. 378-386. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.004.

20. M. Vandermosten; J. Vanderauwera; C. Theys; A. De Vos; S. Vanvooren; S. Sunaert; J. Wouters; P. Ghesquière A DTI tractography study in pre-readers at risk for dyslexia., 2015, 14,pp. 8-15. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.05.006.

21. S. Van Der Auwera; M. Vandermosten; J. Wouters; P. Ghesquière; J. Vanderauwera A three-time point longitudinal investigation of the arcuate fasciculus throughout reading acquisition in children developing dyslexia., 2021, 237,p. 118087. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118087.

22. Y. Wang; M.V. Mauer; T. Raney; B. Peysakhovich; B.L. Becker; D.D. Sliva; N. Gaab Development of tract-specific white matter pathways during early reading development in at-risk children and typical controls., 2017, 27,pp. 2469-2485. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw095.

23. J. Vanderauwera; J. Wouters; M. Vandermosten; P. Ghesquière Early dynamics of white matter deficits in children developing dyslexia., 2017, 27,pp. 69-77. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.08.003.

24. J. Zhao; Z. Song; Y. Zhao; M.T. de Schotten; I. Altarelli; F. Ramus White matter connectivity in uncinate fasciculus accounts for visual attention span in developmental dyslexia., 2022, 177,p. 108414. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2022.108414.

25. S.L. Meisler; J.D. Gabrieli A large-scale investigation of white matter microstructural associations with reading ability., 2022, 249,p. 118909. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.118909.

26. T. Liu; M. Thiebaut de Schotten; I. Altarelli; F. Ramus; J. Zhao Neural dissociation of visual attention span and phonological deficits in developmental dyslexia: A hub-based white matter network analysis., 2022, 43,pp. 5210-5219. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25997.

27. R. Farah; N. Glukhovsky; K. Rosch; T. Horowitz-Kraus Structural white matter characteristics for working memory and switching/inhibition in children with reading difficulties: The role of the left superior longitudinal fasciculus., 2022, 6,pp. 897-915. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00257.

28. M. Partanen; D.H. Kim; A. Rauscher; L.S. Siegel; D.E. Giaschi White matter but not grey matter predicts change in reading skills after intervention., 2021, 27,pp. 224-244. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1668.

29. C. Lou; A.M. Cross; L. Peters; D. Ansari; M.F. Joanisse Rich-club structure contributes to individual variance of reading skills via feeder connections in children with reading disabilities., 2021, 49,p. 100957. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2021.100957.

30. T. Liu; M.T. de Schotten; I. Altarelli; F. Ramus; J. Zhao Maladaptive compensation of right fusiform gyrus in developmental dyslexia: A hub-based white matter network analysis., 2021, 145,pp. 57-66. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2021.07.016.

31. N. Koirala; M.V. Perdue; X. Su; E.L. Grigorenko; N. Landi Neurite density and arborization is associated with reading skill and phonological processing in children., 2021, 241,p. 118426. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118426.

32. E. Huber; A. Mezer; J.D. Yeatman Neurobiological underpinnings of rapid white matter plasticity during intensive reading instruction., 2021, 243,p. 118453. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118453.

33. V. Borghesani; C. Wang; C. Watson; F. Bouhali; E. Caverzasi; G. Battistella; R. Bogley; N.A. Yabut; J. Deleon; Z.A. Miller et al. Functional and morphological correlates of developmental dyslexia: A multimodal investigation of the ventral occipitotemporal cortex., 2021, 31,pp. 962-972. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/jon.12892.

34. C. Vander Stappen; L. Dricot; M. Van Reybroeck RAN training in dyslexia: Behavioral and brain correlates., 2020, 146,p. 107566. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107566.

35. S. El-Sady; S.A. Mohammad; K. Aboualfotouh Ahmed; A.N. Khattab; N.H. Nashaat; G. Orabi; E.R. Abdelraouf Correlation between diffusion tensor imaging measures and the reading and cognitive performance of Arabic readers: Dyslexic children perspective., 2020, 62,pp. 525-531. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00234-020-02368-1.

36. H.L.S. Wang; N.Y.H. Wang; F.C. Yeh Specifying the diffusion MRI connectome in Chinese-speaking children with developmental dyslexia and auditory processing deficits., 2019, 60,pp. 297-304. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pedneo.2018.07.016.

37. J. Vanderauwera; E.R. van Setten; N.M. Maurits; B.A. Maassen The interplay of socio-economic status represented by paternal educational level, white matter structure and reading., 2019, 14, e0215560. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215560.

38. C. Lou; X. Duan; I. Altarelli; J.A. Sweeney; F. Ramus; J. Zhao White matter network connectivity deficits in developmental dyslexia., 2019, 40,pp. 505-516. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24390.

39. C. Lebel; A. Benischek; B. Geeraert; J. Holahan; S. Shaywitz; K. Bakhshi; B. Shaywitz Developmental trajectories of white matter structure in children with and without reading impairments., 2019, 36,p. 100633. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100633.

40. C. Banfi; K. Koschutnig; K. Moll; G. Schulte-Körne; A. Fink; K. Landerl White matter alterations and tract lateralization in children with dyslexia and isolated spelling deficits., 2019, 40,pp. 765-776. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24410.

41. G. Žaric; I. Timmers; P. Gerretsen; G. Fraga González; J. Tijms; M.W. van der Molen; L. Blomert; M. Bonte Atypical white matter connectivity in dyslexic readers of a fairly transparent orthography., 2018, 9,p. 1147. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01147.

42. K. Yagle; T. Richards; K. Askren; Z. Mestre; S. Beers; R. Abbott; W. Nagy; P. Boord; V. Berninger Relationships between eye movements during sentence reading comprehension, word spelling and reading, and DTI and fMRI connectivity in students with and without dysgraphia or dyslexia., 2017,pp. 1-11. DOI: https://doi.org/10.15761/JSIN.1000150.

43. M. Su; J. Zhao; M.T. de Schotten; W. Zhou; G. Gong; F. Ramus; H. Shu Alterations in white matter pathways underlying phonological and morphological processing in Chinese developmental dyslexia., 2018, 31,pp. 11-19. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.002.

44. J.A. Christodoulou; J. Murtagh; A. Cyr; T.K. Perrachione; P. Chang; K. Halverson; P. Hook; A. Yendiki; S. Ghosh; J.D. Gabrieli Relation of white-matter microstructure to reading ability and disability in beginning readers., 2017, 31,p. 508. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000243. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26949926.

45. J. Zhao; M.T. de Schotten; I. Altarelli; J. Dubois; F. Ramus Altered hemispheric lateralization of white matter pathways in developmental dyslexia: Evidence from spherical deconvolution tractography., 2016, 76,pp. 51-62. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.12.004. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26859852.

46. I.K. Koerte; A. Willems; M. Muehlmann; K. Moll; S. Cornell; S. Pixner; D. Steffinger; D. Keeser; F. Heinen; M. Kubicki et al. Mathematical abilities in dyslexic children: A diffusion tensor imaging study., 2016, 10,pp. 781-791. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-015-9436-y.

47. B. Garcia-Zapirain; Y. Garcia-Chimeno; I. Saralegui; B. Fernandez-Ruanova; R. Martinez Differences in effective connectivity between children with dyslexia, monocular vision and typically developing readers: A DTI study., 2016, 23,pp. 19-27. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bspc.2015.07.009.

48. V.G. Fernandez; J. Juranek; A. Romanowska-Pawliczek; K. Stuebing; V.J. Williams; J.M. Fletcher White matter integrity of cerebellar-cortical tracts in reading impaired children: A probabilistic tractography study., 2016, 161,pp. 45-56. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2015.07.006.

49. L.M. De Moura; H. Cogo-Moreira; C.R.B. De Ávila; P.M. Pan; A. Gadelha; T. Moriyama; M.A. Del Aquilla; M. Hoexter; G.A. Salum; F.A. Picon et al. Children with poor reading skills at the word level show reduced fractional anisotropy in white matter tracts of both hemispheres., 2016, 6,pp. 519-523. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2016.0430. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27353747.

50. T.L. Richards; T.J. Grabowski; P. Boord; K. Yagle; M. Askren; Z. Mestre; P. Robinson; O. Welker; D. Gulliford; W. Nagy et al. Contrasting brain patterns of writing-related DTI parameters, fMRI connectivity, and DTI–fMRI connectivity correlations in children with and without dysgraphia or dyslexia., 2015, 8,pp. 408-421. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2015.03.018.

51. C. Marino; P. Scifo; P.A. Della Rosa; S. Mascheretti; A. Facoetti; M.L. Lorusso; R. Giorda; M. Consonni; A. Falini; M. Molteni et al. The DCDC2/intron 2 deletion and white matter disorganization: Focus on developmental dyslexia., 2014, 57,pp. 227-243. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.04.016.

52. Q. Fan; N. Davis; A.W. Anderson; L.E. Cutting Thalamo-cortical connectivity: What can diffusion tractography tell us about reading difficulties in children?., 2014, 4,pp. 428-439. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2013.0203.

53. Q. Fan; A.W. Anderson; N. Davis; L.E. Cutting Structural connectivity patterns associated with the putative visual word form area and children’ s reading ability., 2014, 1586,pp. 118-129. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2014.08.050.

54. K.M. Hasan; D.L. Molfese; I.S. Walimuni; K.K. Stuebing; A.C. Papanicolaou; P.A. Narayana; J.M. Fletcher Diffusion tensor quantification and cognitive correlates of the macrostructure and microstructure of the corpus callosum in typically developing and dyslexic children., 2012, 25,pp. 1263-1270. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/nbm.2797. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22411286.

55. D. Gebauer; C. Enzinger; M. Kronbichler; M. Schurz; G. Reishofer; K. Koschutnig; R. Kargl; C. Purgstaller; F. Fazekas; A. Fink Distinct patterns of brain function in children with isolated spelling impairment: New insights., 2012, 50,pp. 1353-1361. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.02.020. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22401987.

56. F. Hoeft; B.D. McCandliss; J.M. Black; A. Gantman; N. Zakerani; C. Hulme; H. Lyytinen; S. Whitfield-Gabrieli; G.H. Glover; A.L. Reiss et al. Neural systems predicting long-term outcome in dyslexia., 2011, 108,pp. 361-366. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1008950108. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21173250.

57. A.J. Sihvonen; P. Virtala; A. Thiede; M. Laasonen; T. Kujala Structural white matter connectometry of reading and dyslexia., 2021, 241,p. 118411. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118411. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34293464.

58. N. Tschentscher; A. Ruisinger; H. Blank; B. Díaz; K. Von Kriegstein Reduced structural connectivity between left auditory thalamus and the motion-sensitive planum temporale in developmental dyslexia., 2019, 39,pp. 1720-1732. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1435-18.2018. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30643025.

59. D. Moreau; A.J. Wilson; N.S. McKay; K. Nihill; K.E. Waldie No evidence for systematic white matter correlates of dyslexia and dyscalculia., 2018, 18,pp. 356-366. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2018.02.004.

60. C. Müller-Axt; A. Anwander; K. von Kriegstein Altered structural connectivity of the left visual thalamus in developmental dyslexia., 2017, 27,pp. 3692-3698. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.10.034. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29153326.

61. M. Vandermosten; H. Poelmans; S. Sunaert; P. Ghesquière; J. Wouters White matter lateralization and interhemispheric coherence to auditory modulations in normal reading and dyslexic adults., 2013, 51,pp. 2087-2099. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.07.008.

62. C. Lebel; B. Shaywitz; J. Holahan; S. Shaywitz; K. Marchione; C. Beaulieu Diffusion tensor imaging correlates of reading ability in dysfluent and non-impaired readers., 2013, 125,pp. 215-222. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2012.10.009.

63. M. Vandermosten; B. Boets; H. Poelmans; S. Sunaert; J. Wouters; P. Ghesquiere A tractography study in dyslexia: Neuroanatomic correlates of orthographic, phonological and speech processing., 2012, 135,pp. 935-948. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awr363. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22327793.

64. R.E. Frye; J. Liederman; K.M. Hasan; A. Lincoln; B. Malmberg; J. McLean; A. Papanicolaou Diffusion tensor quantification of the relations between microstructural and macrostructural indices of white matter and reading., 2011, 32,pp. 1220-1235. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21103. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20665719.

65. N. Mather; D. Schneider The Use of Cognitive Tests in the Assessment of Dyslexia., 2023, 11, 79. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11050079. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37233328.

66. S. Bruzzone; M. Lumaca; E. Brattico; P. Vuust; M. Kringelbach; L. Bonetti Dissociated brain functional connectivity of fast versus slow frequencies underlying individual differences in fluid intelligence: A DTI and MEG study., 2022, 12,p. 4746. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-08521-5. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35304521.

67. E. Huber; P.M. Donnelly; A. Rokem; J.D. Yeatman Rapid and widespread white matter plasticity during an intensive reading intervention., 2018, 9,p. 2260. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-04627-5. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29884784.

68. C.R. Buchanan; S. Muñoz Maniega; M.C. Valdés Hernández; L. Ballerini; G. Barclay; A.M. Taylor; T.C. Russ; E.M. Tucker-Drob; J.M. Wardlaw; I.J. Deary et al. Comparison of structural MRI brain measures between 1.5 and 3 T: Data from the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936., 2021, 42,pp. 3905-3921. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25473. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34008899.

69. M. Lazar; J.H. Jensen; L. Xuan; J.A. Helpern Estimation of the orientation distribution function from diffusional kurtosis imaging., 2008, 60,pp. 774-781. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.21725. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18816827.

70. G.R. Glenn; J.A. Helpern; A. Tabesh; J.H. Jensen Optimization of white matter fiber tractography with diffusional kurtosis imaging., 2015, 28,pp. 1245-1256. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/nbm.3374. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26275886.

71. G.R. Glenn; L.W. Kuo; Y.P. Chao; C.Y. Lee; J.A. Helpern; J.H. Jensen Mapping the orientation of white matter fiber bundles: A comparative study of diffusion tensor imaging, diffusional kurtosis imaging, and diffusion spectrum imaging., 2016, 37,pp. 1216-1222. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3174/ajnr.A4714.

72. G. Barrio-Arranz; R. de Luis-García; A. Tristán-Vega; M. Martín-Fernández; S. Aja-Fernández Impact of MR acquisition parameters on DTI scalar indexes: A tractography based approach., 2015, 10, e0137905. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137905.

73. S.S. Bao; C. Zhao; X.X. Bao; J.S. Rao; J. Rao Effect of Value on Imaging Quality for Diffusion Tensor Imaging of the Spinal Cord at Ultrahigh Field Strength., 2021, 2021, 4836804. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1155/2021/4836804. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33506018.

74. L.P. Qi; Z. Zhong; Y.S. Sun; X.T. Li; L. Tang; X.J. Zhou Optimal selection of b-values for differential diagnosis of mediastinal lymph nodes using diffusion-weighted imaging., 2023, 9,p. e16702. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2023.e16702. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37484276.

75. T. Sartoretti; E. Sartoretti; M. Wyss; M. Mannil; L. van Smoorenburg; B. Eichenberger; C. Reischauer; A. Alfieri; C. Binkert; S. Sartoretti-Schefer Diffusion-weighted MRI of ischemic stroke at 3T: Value of synthetic b-values., 2021, 94,p. 20200869. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1259/bjr.20200869. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33596102.

76. V. Kumpulainen; H. Merisaari; A. Copeland; E. Silver; E.P. Pulli; J.D. Lewis; E. Saukko; J. Saunavaara; L. Karlsson; H. Karlsson et al. Effect of number of diffusion-encoding directions in diffusion metrics of 5-year-olds using tract-based spatial statistical analysis., 2022, 56,pp. 4843-4868. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.15785.

77. F.W. Hoefnagels; P.C. de Witt Hamer; P.J. Pouwels; F. Barkhof; W.P. Vandertop Impact of gradient number and voxel size on diffusion tensor imaging tractography for resective brain surgery., 2017, 105,pp. 923-934. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wneu.2017.06.050. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28624562.

78. B. Jeurissen; M. Descoteaux; S. Mori; A. Leemans Diffusion MRI fiber tractography of the brain., 2019, 32, e3785. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/nbm.3785. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28945294.

79. J.R. Schlaier; A.L. Beer; R. Faltermeier; C. Fellner; K. Steib; M. Lange; M.W. Greenlee; A.T. Brawanski; J.M. Anthofer Probabilistic vs. deterministic fiber tracking and the influence of different seed regions to delineate cerebellar-thalamic fibers in deep brain stimulation., 2017, 45,pp. 1623-1633. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13575. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28391647.

80. D. Christiaens; M. Reisert; T. Dhollander; S. Sunaert; P. Suetens; F. Maes Global tractography of multi-shell diffusion-weighted imaging data using a multi-tissue model., 2015, 123,pp. 89-101. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.008. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26272729.

81. N. Bodammer; J. Kaufmann; M. Kanowski; C. Tempelmann Eddy current correction in diffusion-weighted imaging using pairs of images acquired with opposite diffusion gradient polarity., 2004, 51,pp. 188-193. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.10690. PMID: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14705060.

82. S. Oldham; A. Arnatkeviciute; R. Smith; J. Tiego; M. Bellgrove; A. Fornito The efficacy of different preprocessing steps in reducing motion-related confounds in diffusion MRI connectomics., 2020, 222,p. 117252. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117252.

Figures and Tables

Figure 1: PRISMA flowchart of this systematic review study, identifying at each stage the number of studies included and the reasons for excluding studies until the final stage of inclusion of studies. [Please download the PDF to view the image]

Figure 2: Spider graphic of the diffusion tensor image (DTI) outcomes percentage distributed by the main tracts reported in the systematic review and their DTI metrics behavior found according to tract and age group of dyslexia participants (pre-reader children in green, reader children in red, and reader adults in purple). The arrows indicate the increase or decrease in DTI metrics in the dyslexia group in comparison to the control group. Abbreviations: AF: arcuate fasciculus; SLF: superior longitudinal fasciculus; IFOF: inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus; CC: corpus callosum; UF: uncinate fasciculus; ThR: thalamic radiations; ILF: inferior longitudinal fasciculus; CG: cingulate cortex; CS: corticospinal fasciculus; CR: corona radiata; OR: optic radiation; Forceps: forceps major and minor; VOF: ventral occipital fasciculus; FA: fractional anisotropy; AD: axial diffusivity; MD: mean diffusivity; QA: quantitative anisotropy; RA: relative anisotropy; and HMOA: hindrance-modulated oriented anisotropy. [Please download the PDF to view the image]

Table 1: Demographic and neuropsychological assessment.

Ref.YearCountryLanguageGroupNSex (F:M)Age (Years)Years of Education (or Level)IQA Word Reading/SpellingPseudoword ReadingText ReadingRANPhonological AwarenessLanguageAttentionOthers

Zuk J, et al. [16]

2021

USA

English

TR FHD- TR FHD+ RD FHD+

39 18 17

21:18 7:11 9:8

5.5 ± 0.3 5.5 ± 0.3 5.7 ± 0.4

Pre-Kindergarten; Kindergarten

Non-verbal (KBIT-2)

LWID (WRMT-R/NU and WRMT-R); Letter Sound Knowledge (YARC)

NR

NR

Objects;Colors; Letter

Elision;Blending Words(CTOPP)

Vocabulary Knowledge (PPVT-4); Sentence Comprehension (CELF-4); Speed Accuracy

NR

WM: Nonword Repetition (CTOPP), Sentence Repetition (GAPS)

Yu X, et al. [17]

2020

USA

English

TR FHD- TR FHD+ RD FHD+

34 35 12

16:18 17:18 4:8

5.4 ± 0.3 5.5 ± 0.4 5.8 ± 0.5

The end of 1st grade to 4th/ grade

Non-verbal (KBIT-2)

WID (WRMT-R);SWE (TOWRE-2)

PDE (TOWRE-2);WA (WRMT-R)

NR

Objects; Colors

CTOPP

CELF-4

NR

HLE

Langer N, et al. [18]

2017

USA

English

FHD+ FHD-

14 18

7:7 10:8

0.9 ± 0.3 0.8 ± 0.3

NR

NR

NA

NA

NA

NA

NA

Expressive and receptive language (MSEL)

NA

Gross and fine motor (MSEL); Visual reception (MSEL)

Kraft I, et al. [19]

2016

Germany

German

FHD+ FHD-

25 28

11:14 12:16

5.7 ± 0.4 5.6 ± 0.4

Kindergarten

Non-verbal

One minuteword reading (SLRT-II);Spelling (DERET)

One minutepseudoword reading(SLRT-II)

NR

Subtest (BISC)

Pseudoword repetition (SETK 3-5), SS and RI (BISC), and PA (BAKO)

NR

Symbol comparison (BISC)

DS (K-ABC)

Vandermosten M, et al. [20]

2015

Belgium

Dutch

FRD+ FRD-

36 35

13:23 17:18

5.1 ± 0.2 5.1 ± 0.2

The last year of kindergarten

Non-verbal (Raven)

Letter knowledge productive/receptive test

NR

NR

Objects; Colors

End-phoneme and end-rhyme identification task (PA)

NR

NR

NR

Van Der Auwera S, et al. [21]

2021

Belgium

Dutch

PreR: FRD-FRD+BR: FRD-FRD+FR: FRD-FRD+

24 16 13 24 10 15

14:10 8:8 13:10 12:12 2:8 5:10

6 ± 0.1 6 ± 0.1 8 ± 0.1 8 ± 0.1 11 ± 0.1 11 ± 0.2

Kindergarten1st/ 2nd grades 3rd/4th/5th grades

Non-verbal (Raven and Block Design– WISC-III)

Word ReadingList; Letterknowledge; andSpelling

Pseudoword Reading Test

NR

NR

Phoneme Deletion and Spoonerism; PA

NR

NR

NR

Wang Y, et al. [22]

2017

USA

English

PreR: FHD-FHD+BR: FHD-FHD+FR: FHD-FHD+

16 24 23 24 10 15

8:8 14:10 12:12 13:10 2:8 5:10

5.3 ± 0.8 5.4 ± 1.1 7.0 ± 2.2 7.3 ± 2.2 10.0 ± 2.5 10.2 ± 1.8

Nine single words 1st/2nd grades 3rd/4th/5th grades

Non-verbal (KBIT-2)

WID (WRMT-R)(FR); SWE (TOWRE)(BR and FR); andTOSWRF (FR)

PDE(TOWRE)

Gray Oral Reading Test(GORT-5), Reading Fluency WJ-III-TA

Objects (PreR);Colors

CTOPP (FR);PC (WRMT-R, BR)

CELF-4 (BR)

NR

TOMAL2 (FR)

Vanderauwera J, et al. [23]

2017

Belgium

Dutch

DYX TR FRD+ FRD-

15 46 34 27

7:8 15:31 13:21 9:18

7.9 ± 0.1 7.9 ± 0.1 7.9 ± 0.1 7.9 ± 0.1

PreR—prior 1st grade/ BR—2nd/3rd grade

Non-verbal (WISC)

Word reading, one-minute test (BR); Spelling (BR); and Productive/Receptive Letter Knowledge (PreR)

Pseudoword readingtwo minutetest (BR)

NR

Objects (PreR);Colors

PA (PreR);End-phoneme andend-rhymeidentification task

NA

NA

NR

Zhao J, et al. [24]

2022

France

French

Control DYX

31 26

13:18 13:13

12 ± 1:11 ± 2 11 ± 1:12 ± 1

NR

Verbal (WISC);Non-verbal (WISC)

Word ReadingAbility (Odedys);Word Spelling-to-Dictation Test

Nonword ReadingAbility (Odedys)

AlouetteTest

Digits; Objects

Phoneme deletion and Spoonerism

NR

VAS (Globaland PartialLetter Report Task)

Verbal WM(DS- WISC)

Meisler SL, [25]

2022

USA

English

Control DYX

582 104

195:387 44:60

10.8 ± 3.2 10.2 ± 2.5

NR

Non-verbal (KBIT-2)

SWE (TOWRE-2)

PDE (TOWRE-2)

NR

NR

NR

NR

NR

NR

Liu T, et al. [26]

2022

France

French

Control DYX

31 16

13:18 13:13

11 ± 1 12 ± 1

NR

Verbal and Non-verbal (WISC-IV)

NR; Global and Partial Letter Report Task (VAS)

NR

NR

Digits; Objects

Phoneme deletionand Spoonerism

NR

NR

Verbal WM (DS- WISC)

Farah R, et al. [27]

2022

USA

English

Control RD

24 22

12:12 10:12

8-12

NR

Non-verbal (TONI); Verbal (PPVT)

SWE (TOWRE);LWID (WJ-III)

PDE (TOWRE);WA (WJ-III)

NR

NR

Elision(CTOPP)

NR

Conners questionnaires and VSA (TEA-Ch)

DS (WISC); Switching/Inhibition (DKEFS, Color-Word Condition); and Overall EF (BRIEF)

Partanen M, et al. [28]

2020

Canada

English

Control DYX

22 13

11:11 5:8

Pre-test 8.5 ± 0.4 8.6 ± 0.4 Post-test 8.9 ± 0.4 8.9 ± 0.4

3rd Grade

Non-verbal (TONI-4)

Word Recognition Task (KTEA-II)

Decoding Task(KTEA-II)

Reading Comprehension (KTEA-II)

NR

NR

NR

NR

NR

Lou C, et al. [29]

2020

Canada

English

RD random group

64

33:31:00

10.9 ± 1.3

NR

NR

SWE (TOWRE)

PDE (TOWRE)

Reading Comprehension (WJ III)

Letters

NR

NR

NR

NR

Liu T, et al. [30]

2021

France

French

Control DYX

31 26

13:18 13:13

11 ± 1 12 ± 1

NR

Verbal andNon-verbal (WISC)

Word Reading Fluency (Odedys);Word Spelling-to-Dictation Test

Nonword ReadingFluency (Odedys)

Alouette Test

NR

NR

NR

NR

NR

Koirala N, et al. [31]

2021

USA

English

Random group

244

151:34:00

10.2 ± 2.8

NR

FSIQ(WISC)

SWE (TOWRE-2)

PDE (TOWRE-2)

NR

NR

Elision andBlending Words(CTOPP-2)

NR

NR

NR

Huber E, et al. [32]

2021

USA

English

Control DYX

41 32

16:25 12:20

9.4 9.8

NR

NR

SWE (TOWRE)

PDE (TOWRE)

WJ-RF

NR

NR

NR

NR

WJ-MFF; WJ-CALC; and WJ-BRS

Borghesani V, et al. [33]

2021

USA

English

Control DYX

14 26

5:9 14:12

10.4 ± 1.6 10.4 ± 2.0

1st grade and 4th grade

Non-verbal (WASI)

SWE (TOWRE-2)

PDE (TOWRE-2)

Gray Oral Reading Test (GORT-5)

NR

NR

NR

NR

NA

Vander Stappen C, et al. [34]

2020

France

French

Control DYX

13 18

5:8 9:9

10.5 ± 0.8 10.6 ± 1.0

NR

Non-verbal (WISC-IV)

SWE - BALE

BALE

BALE

Objects; Colors

Syllable andphoneme deletion task

NR

NR

NR

El-Sady S, et al. [35]

2020

Egypt

Arabic

DYX

20

05:15

8.2 ± 1

NR

SB4

1 min reading DAT

nonsense passage reading DAT

1 min reading DAT

Objects

Phonemic segmentation subtest of DAT

NR

NR

Bead threading; Postural stability; and DS

Wang HLS, et al. [36]

2019

Taiwan

Mandarin Chinese

Control DYX

22 24

NR

9 ± 0.9 10 ± 1

Primary school

Non-verbal (WISC-IV)

Chinese character recognition

NR

NR

NR

NR

NR

NR

Lexical toneawareness;auditoryidentificationof FM test

Vanderauwera J, et al. [37]

2019

Netherlands

Dutch

TR and RD

34

19:15

13.7 ± 0.5

grade 8 (28), 7 (3) and 9 (3)

WISC-III-NL

One-minute word reading test

Klepel test

NR

NA

NA

NA

NA

NR

Lou C, et al. [38]

2019

France

French

Control DYX

31 26

13:18 13:13

11.5 ± 1.4 11.6 ± 1.3

NR

Verbal andNon-verbal (WISC)

Word readingtest (Odedys)

Nonwordreading test(Odedys)

Alouette test

Digits;Objects

Phoneme deletionand spoonerism

Word spelling-to-dictation test

NR

Verbal WM(DS- WISC)

Lebel C, et al. [39]

2019

USA

English

Dysfluent inaccurate Dysfluent accurate Non-impaired

20 36 14

5:15 13:23 8:6

10.0 ± 1.2 9.4 ± 1.3 9.2 ± 1.2

NR

WASI FSIQ

SWE (TOWRE);LWID (WJ)

PDE (TOWRE);WA (WJ)

Gray OralReadingTest(GORT-4)

NR

PhonologicalDecoding(TOWRE)

NR

NR

NR

Banfi C, et al. [40]

2018

Austria

German

TR DYX SD

27 21 21

12:15 9:12 6:15

9 ± 0.1 9 ± 0.3 10 ± 0.6

The end of 3rd and 4th grade

Verbal and Non-verbal (WISC)

(SRLT-II);Spelling (DRT-3)

(SRLT-II)

Sentencereadingfluency(SLS)

Digits;Objects

PA

Vocabulary standard score (WISC-IV)

Parentalquestionnaire ADHD

Verbal WM and processing speed (DS, Symbol search WISC-IV)

Žaric G, et al. [41]

2018

Netherlands

Dutch

TR DYX

13 15

8:5 7:8

9 ± 0.8 9 ± 0.6

2–3 years of reading instruction

Non-verbal (WISC)

Word readingsubtest (3DM)

Pseudowordreading subtest (3DM)

NR

Letters;Digits;Objects

Phoneme deletion; Spelling;and Letter speechsound matching

NR

NR

Memory span (syllables)

Su M, et al. [43]

2018

China

Mandarin Chinese

Control DYX

22 18

11:11 7:11

11 ± 0.8 11 ± 1.0

Primary school

Non-verbal and Verbal (C-WISC)

Word listreading; Chinesecharacter recognition

NR

NR

Digits

Phoneme deletion

Lexical decision;Morphologicalproduction

NR

Verbal WM(Digit recall)

Yagle K, et al. [42]

2017

USA

English

TR DYG DYX

10 9 10

NR

9–14

4–9 grades

Non-Verbal (Wechsler)

Word reading(TOSWRF); wordspelling (TOC)

Nonword reading

NR

NR

NR

NR

NR

NR

Christodoulou JA, et al. [44]

2016

USA

English

TR DYX

26 26

NR

7.8 ± 0.6 7.8 ± 0.6

NR

Non-verbal (KBIT-2)

WID (WRMT-III);SWE (TOWRE-2)

WA (WRMT-III);PDE (TOWRE-2)

NR

NR

NR

NR

NR

NR

Zhao JT, et al. [45]

2016

France

French

Control DYX

31 26

13:18 13:13

11.5 ± 1.3 11.6 ± 1.3

NR

Verbal andNon-verbal (WISC)

Word readingfluency (Odedys);Word spelling

Nonword readingfluency (Odedys)

Alouette Test

Digits, Objects

Word spelling-to-dictation test, Spoonerism

NR

NR

DS (WISC)

Koerte IK, et al. [46]

2015

Germany

German

Control DYX

24 16

0:24 0:16

9.9 ± 0.3 9.7 ± 0.4

3rd and 4th grades

Non-verbal (CFT-20R)

SLRT-II

SLRT-II

NR

Digits, Letters,Colors, Objects

Phonemedeletion

NR

NR

DS (K-ABC); Verbal WM (Wechsler); Arithmetic test (HRT 1-4); and Number line task (WRT 1-4)

Garcia-Zapirain BG, et al. [47]

2016

Spain

Spanish

TR DYX MVR

19 20 18

8:11 8:12 8:10

10.0 ± 0.9 10.5 ± 1.1 10.4 ± 0.9

NR

Verbal andNon-verbal (WISC-IV)

Word reading (PROLEC-R)

Pseudowordreading (PROLEC-R)

ELFE 1-6

NR

NR

NR

NR

WM

Fernandez VG, et al. [48]

2016

USA

English

TR DYX

27 29

15:12 14:15

10.1 ± 2.1 12.1 ± 2.5

6–8 grades

Verbal andnon-verbal (KBIT-2, SB4)

LWI (WJ-III);WRAT-3; andSWE (TOWRE)

PDE (TOWRE)

PC (WJ-III-TA)

NR

NR

NR

NR

NR

De Moura LM, et al. [49]

2016

Brazil

Portuguese

TR RD

23 17

12:11 9:8

9.7 ± 0.9 9.2 ± 0.9

NR

Verbal and Non-verbal (WISC-III)

Aloud reading(TDE)

NR

NR

NR

NR

NR

NR

NR

Richards TL, et al. [50]

2015

USA

English

Control DYX DYG

9 17 14

5:4 7:10 3:11

mean of 12.25 (from 9 to 15.6)

4–9 grades

Verbal(WISC-IV)

Spelling dictatedwords (WIAT III);Sight Spelling (TOC)

NR

NR

NR

NR

NR

NR

Best and Fastwriting (DASH)

Marino C, et al. [51]

2014

Italy

Italian

TR FRD+ TR FRD- DYX FRD+ DYX FRD-

10 16 11 10

5:5 6:10 6:5 4:6

19.1 ± 1.9 18.7 ± 2.4 17.5 ± 2.4 16.4 ± 1.0

12.8 ± 1.5 12.0 ± 1.1 10.2 ± 1.8 10.8 ± 1.2

Full-scale IQ (WISC-R)

Word reading(BVDDE);Spelling (BVDDE)

Non-wordreading (BVDDE)

Sentencescontaininghom*ophones

NR

Spoonerism,phonemic blending, andsyllable displacement (PA)

NR

NR

ADC, letter andnumber forward/backwardspan (TEMA)

Fan Q, et al. [52]

2014

USA

English

Control DYX

20 19

9:11 8:11

12.0 ± 0.7 12.0 ± 0.7

NR

Verbal andNon-verbal(WISC-IV)

LWID (WJ-III);SWE (TOWRE); andFLI and spelling(WIST)

WA (WJ-III);PDE (TOWRE)

PC and basicreading (WJ-III);TOSCRF

Color DigitObjects (CTOPP)

NR

NR

NR

NR

Fan Q, et al. [53]

2014

USA

English

TR RD

16 20

8:8 8:12

11.7 ± 0.7 12.1 ± 0.7

NR

Verbal andNon-verbal(WISC-IV)

LWID (WJ-III);SWE (TOWRE);and FLI (WIST)

WA (WJ-III);PDE (TOWRE)

NR

NR

WJ-III-PC

NR

NR

NR

Hasan KM, et al. [54]

2012

USA

English

TR DYX CFP

11 24 15

3:8 11:1 39:6

12.8 ± 1.7 13.7 ± 1.0 13.5 ± 0.8

NR

Composite IQ(KBIT-2, SB4)

LWID (WJ-III);SWE (TOWRE)

PDE (TOWRE)

PC (WJ-III)

NR

NR

NR

NR

NR

Gebauer D, et al. [55]

2012

Austria

German

Control SI SRI

11 11 9

NR

12.3 ± 1.9 11.7 ± 1.6 11.3 ± 0.7

4th–5th 5–9 graders

Non-verbal (Raven)

SLS 1-4 or 5-8;Spelling (HSP)

SLS 1-4or 5-8

ELFE 1-6

NR

NR

NR

NR

Personality assessment FFQ (Asendropf)

Hoeft F, et al. [56]

2011

USA

English

Control DYX (rg) DYX (nrg)

20 13 12

14:6 6:7 7:5

11.0 ± 2.6 14.5 ± 1.6 13.5 ± 2.2

NR

Non-verbal(WASI)

(WRMT) *;SWE (TOWRE);and Spelling andwriting fluency (WJ)

WA (WRMT);PDE (TOWRE)

Gray OralReadingTest(GORT);PC (WRMT)

Colors; Objects;Numbers; and Letters

NR

PPVT

NR

MD (CTOPP)

Sihvonen AJ, et al. [57]

2021

Finland

Finnish

Control DYX

21 23

11:10 12:11

29.9 ± 6.0 31.3 ± 8.6

16.1 ± 4.4 15.7 ± 5.2

Verbal (WAIS-III);PIQ (WAIS-IV)

Word ListReading

PseudowordList Reading

Text Reading

Test notspecified

Pig Latin;PA; phonologicalshort-term memory;and rapid accessof information

NR

ASRS v1.1

ARHQ;Verbal WM (Non-word Span Length, WMS-III)

Tschentscher N, et al. [58]

2019

Germany

German

Control DYX

12 12

0:12 0:12

23.7 ± 2.6 24.2 ± 2.3

NR

Nonverbal (Raven)

Spelling

NR

Reading speedandcomprehension

Letters andNumbers

NR

NR

NR

NR

Moreau D, et al. [59]

2018

New Zealand

English

Control Dyscalc DYX Comorbid

11 11 11 12

4:7 5:6 5:6 5:7

27.7 ± 1.7 32 ± 2.2 29.4 ± 1.9 33.2 ± 1.7

15.2 ± 0.60 14.6 ± 0.56 15.6 ± 0.51 14.8 ± 0.61

FSIQ (WASI)

WID (WJ)

WA (WJ)

NR

NR

NR

WRAT spelling

NR

WRAT mathematics

Müller-Axt C, et al. [60]

2017

Germany

German

Control DYX

12 12

0:12 0:12

23.7 ± 2.6 24.2 ± 2.4

Undergraduate students **

Non-verbal (Raven)

Spelling

NR

NR

Numbers;Letters

NR

NR

NR

NR

Vandermosten M, et al. [61]

2013

Belgium

Dutch

TR DYX

20 20

12:8 13:7

21.4 ± 3.0 22.1 ± 3.1

NR

Non-verbal (WAIS-III)

Word reading;Spelling

Pseudowordreading

NR

NR

NR

NR

NR

NR

Lebel C, et al. [62]

2013

USA

English

RLD

136

64:13:00

20.1 ± 3.1

NR

FSIQ (WASI)

WID (WJ)

WA (WJ)

Fluency(GORT)

NR

NR

NR

NR

NR

Vandermosten M, et al. [63]

2012

Belgium

Dutch

TR DYX

20 20

12:8 13:7

21.4 ± 3.0 22.1 ± 3.1

NR

Non-verbal (WAIS)

Word reading;Spelling

Pseudowordreading

NR

NR

PA; Phonemedeletion andSpoonerism

Speech-in- noise perception (Dutch LIST)

NR

NR

Frye RE, et al. [64]

2011

USA

English

TR DYX/PR

20 10

10:10 5:5

23.7 ± 0.7 23.9 ± 1.6

NR

Non-verbal(CTONI)

LWI (WJ-III);Spelling

WA (WJ-III)

Gray OralReadingTest(GORT)

Colors (CTOPP);Digits (CTOPP);Objects (CTOPP);Letters (CTOPP)

PA (CTOPP);APA (CTOPP)

NR

Test of variables of attention: commissions, omissions

NR

Note: Bold font indicates significant group differences. * time effect in DYX group, ** only 1 control had a high school diploma. Abbreviations: NA: Not Applicable; NR: Not Reported; FRD+: Children with Familial Risk for Dyslexia; FRD-: Children without Familial Risk for Dyslexia; TR: Typical reading; rg: reading gain; nrg: no rg; m: months; RAN: rapid automatized naming tasks; DYX: children with dyslexia; RD: Reading Disorder; RI: Reading Impairment; PreR: pre-reader children; BR: older reader children; FR: fluent reader children; PIQ: Performance IQ; FSIQ: Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient; CFT 20-R: Cattell’s Fluid Intelligence Test, Scale 2; TrR: Treatment Responders; EF: Executive Functions; MD: Mood Disorders; WRD: word recognition deficits; RLD: reading and learning disabilities; SI: spelling impaired children; SRI: children with spelling and reading impairment; CFP: readers with comprehension or fluency problems; PA: Phonological awareness; PPVT: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; TONI or TONI-4: Test of Nonverbal Intelligence; CTONI: Comprehensive TONI; CTOPP: Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing; TOWRE or TOWRE-2: Test of Word Reading Efficiency; WJ: the Reading Fluency subtest of the Woodco*ck-Johnson Test; WISC or WISC-IV: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 4th edition; WASI: Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence; WAIS: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd edition; WIAT: Wechsler Individual Achievement Test; BRIEF: Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function; KBIT or KBIT-2: Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test; WRMT-R NU: Woodco*ck Reading Mastery Tests-Revised, Normative Update; WA: Word attention; SLRT-II: The Salzburg Reading and Spelling Test; YARC: York Assessment of Reading for Comprehension; GAPS: Grammar and Phonology Screening; CELF or CELF-4: Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals; KTEA-II: Kaufman Test of Educational Achievement-Second Edition; CVLT: California Verbal Learning Test; WID: word identification; LWID: letter and WID; VAS: Visual Attention Span; SB4: Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales-Fouth Edition; HLE: Home Literacy Environment; HOME: the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment; DAT: Dyslexia Assessment Test; ASRS: Adult Self Report Scale for ADHD clinical assessment; TOC: Test of Orthographic Competence; TOMAL2: Test of Memory and Learning - Second Edition; MSEL: Mullen Scales of Early Learning; ARHQ: Adult Reading History Questionnaire; ADC: Adult Dyslexia Checklist; HSP: Hamburger-Schreibprobe; SLS: Salzburger-Lese-Sreening; FFQ: five factor questionnaire; DAWBA: Diagnostic andWell-Being Assessment; SWE: SightWord Efficiency; DS: digit span; VSA: visual spatial attention; BISC: Bielefeld screening of literacy precursor abilities; DERET: German spelling test; SETK 3 5: a developmental German language test for children between 3 and 5 years of age; BAKO: Test of basic reading and spelling skills; PDE: Phonemic Decoding Efficiency; TOSWRF or TOSWRF-2: Test of Silent Word Reading Fluency, Second Edition; GORT: Grey Oral Reading Test; PC: Passage comprehension; ODEDYS: dyslexia screening tool; DKEFS: Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System; BALE: Analytic Battery of Written Language; DRT-3: Spelling percentile; 3DM: differential diagnostics for dyslexia; HRT: Heidelberger Rechentest; WRT: Weingartener Grundwortschatz Rechtschreibtest; PROLEC: Text Comprehension task; WRAT: Wide Range Achievement Test; TDE: test for School Achievement; DASH: Detailed Assessment of Speed of Handwriting; FLI: Fundamental Literacy Index; WIST: Word Identification and Spelling Test; ELFE: standardized achievement tests; TEMA: Test di Memoria e Apprendimento; BVDDE: Battery for the Assessment of Developmental Reading and Spelling Disabilities; and WM: working memory.

Table 2: DTI acquisition, image processing, and outcomes.

DTI AcquisitionDTI ProcessingDTI Outcomes
RefMRI FieldSequenceTR/TE (ms)Slice NumberSlice Thickness (mm)FOV (mm)b-Value (s/mm[sup.2])N. of Diffusion GradientsTimeSoftwareCorrectionsType ofAnalysesDTIMetricsAtlasROIs/ TRACTSTracts Differencebetween GroupsClinicalCorrelations

Zuk J, et al. [16]

Siemens 3T

DTI

NR

30

2

128x128

0; 700

NR

NR

DTIprep, VISTALab

EC, HM (>2 mm or >0.5°)

ROI

FA

NR

AF, SLF

?FA in r-SLF of FHD+ TR compared to FHD- TR and FHD+ RD

r-SLF FA, age, gender, parent education, occupation, and phonological awareness significantly predicted decoding skills among children FHD+

Yu X, et al. [17]

Siemens 3T

DTI

NR

NR

2

NR

0; 700; 1000

NR

NR

DTIprep, VISTALab, AFQ

EC, HM (>2 mm/0.5°), bed vibration, pulsation, venetian blind artifacts, and slice and gradient-wise intensity inconsistencies

Whole brain; ROI

FA

MNI, Native space

Right of SLF, ILF, and AF, sCC, CC2

?FA in r-sCC of FHD+ TR compared to FHD- TR

R-sCC FA had positive correlation with r-IFG activation for FHD-/+ TR

Langer N, et al. [18]

Siemens 3T

DTI

8320/88

64

2

256x256

1000

30

5:59 min

DTIprep, FSL (DTIFIT), Trackvis (Diffusion Toolkit), Trackvis, AFQ

EC and HM (>2 mm and 0.5°)

Whole brain; ROI

FA RD AD

MNI

Bilateral AF and CS

?FA in l-AF (central portion) FHD+ compared with FHD-, corrected by age

l-AF FA has positive correlation with age, expressive language

Kraft I, et al. [19]

Siemens 3T

DTI

8000/NR

66

1.9

NR

1000

60

32 min

FSL (Topup tool), FSL (DTIFIT), MRTrix

EC, HM, and susceptibility- induced distortions

ROI

FA

Destrieux Atlas

SMG, ITG (anterior, long, and posterior AF), SOS/TOS, IFoG, IFobG

No group difference

l-aAF was the best predictor of DYX

Vanderm- osten M, et al. [20]

Philips 3T

DTI

7600/65

58

2.5

200x240

1300

60

10 min 32 s

Explore DTI, Trackvis

EC, HM (6 parameters) Reorientation of the b-matrix Motion as covariate

Whole brain; ROI

FA

TrackVis

AF (dorsal FTP, dorsal post TP), ventral IFOF

?FA in l-IFOF of FHD+

Phonological awareness positive correlation with FA of l-AF(TP) and bilateral IFOF/AF-FTP, as also left ventral tracts in FHD+

Van Der Auwera S, et al. [21]

Philips 3T

DTI

7600/65

NR

2.5

NR

1300

60

10:32 min

FSL, VISTALab, AFQ

EC, HM by root mean square

Whole brain; ROI

FA MD

NR

AF

?FA in the l-AF in pre-reader RDs

aAF FA was a significant predictor for scores on word reading tests from 2nd grade

Wang Y, et al. [22]

Siemens 3T

DTI

8320/88

NR

NR

256x256

1000

30

5:59 min

DTIprep, VISTALab, AFQ

EC, HM (>2 mm and >0.5°)

Whole brain; ROI

FA AD RD

white matter atlas

Left of AF, SLF, ILF

?l-AF FA at pre-reader FHD+ versus FHD- and for poor versus good readers all ages; FHD+ good readers had faster WM development in r-SLF compared to poor readers

l-AF and ILF FA positive correlations with word identification skill

Vanderau- wera J, et al. [23]

Philips 3T

DTI

7600/65

NR

2.5

NR

1300

60

10:32 min

ExploreDTI, Trackvis

EC, HM

ROI

FA

native space

Long, anterior and posterior dorsal AF, and ventral IFOF

?FA in all groups over time. ? long AF FA in DYX prior to reading onset, right also kept in early reading. Influence of FHD+ in l-IFOF and long r-AF

FHD+ and rapid naming predicted 80.3% of cases; the l-longAF FA values predicted 84.4% of DYX cases

Zhao J. et al. [24]

Siemens 3T

DTI

14,000/91

70

1.7

218

1400

60

18 min

Explore DTI, Trackvis, FSL

NR

Whole brain

FA

TrackVis MNI-152

UF, FAT

Males DYX had a ?HMOA in the UF compared with males TR

HMOA of the UF showed a positive correlation with VAS in DYXs

Meisler SL, [25]

Siemens 3T

DKI

3320/100.2

NR

1.8

NR

0; 1000; 2000

64

NR

QSIPrep, MRtrix, FSL, and TractSeg

Gibbs unringing, EC, HM, and AP-PA field

Whole brain

FA

FSL and MNI

AF, SLF (I, II, and III), ILF, IFOF, UF, SCP, ICP, MCP, and sCC

No group difference

Age and sex with gFA positive correlation; in older children, FA in r-SLF and l-ICP related to nonword reading skills

Liu T, et al. [26]

Siemens 3T

DTI

14,000/91

70

1.7

218

1400

60

18 min

PANDA, FSL, and Trackvis

EC

Whole brain

FA

MNI and AAL atlas

90 ROIs of AAL

NR

Positive correlation between node FA for l-SOG and VAS score, l-MTG and l-ORBsupmed and phonological score

Farah R, et al. [27]

Philips 3T

DTI

6652.446/82.6

160

2

224x120x224

1000

61

7 min 25 s

VISTALab, AFQ

EC, HM

Whole brain; ROI

FA

NR

AF, SLF, ILF

? FA in the left of AF, ILF, and SLF in RD

? FA in the l-SLF positive correlated with reading and working memory score in DYX

Partanen M, et al. [28]

GE 3T

DTI

7000/60

60

2

256x256

0; 1000

60

7.5 min

TORTOISE, FDT (FSL), DTIFIT (FSL), and PROBTRACKX (FSL)

EC, HM

Whole brain; ROI

FA MD

MNI305 and Desikan–Killiany atlas

bilateral IFG, Ins, STG, SMG, AnG, and FFG

?MD in bilateral Ins; l-IFtG, l-STG, and r-SMG in DYX

SMG, r-IFoG, and l-Ins MD had negative correlation with reading gains and decoding, respectively

Lou C, et al. [29]

Siemens 3T

DTI

3000/50.6

64

2

256x256

0; 1000

56

NR

ExploreDTI

EC, HM,EPI distortions

Whole brain; ROI

FA

AAL and MNI152

90 ROIs of AAL

NR

IFtG and IFoG, Ins, FFG, IPL, SMG, AnG, HG, STG, MTG, ITG, IOG, PreCG, ROL, and thalamus in the left hemisphere positive correlated with reading efficiency and phonemic decoding, mainly for girls DYX

Liu T, et al. [30]

Siemens 3T

DTI

14,000/91

70

1.7

218x218

0; 1400

60

18 min

PANDA, FSL

EC, HM

Whole brain; ROI

FA

AAL and MNI

90 ROIs of AAL

NR

Negative correlation between READACC (pseudoword/word reading) and the r-FFG FA in DYX

Koirala N, et al. [31]

Siemens 3T

DTI

NR

NR

1.8

NR

0; 1000; 2000

64

NR

FSL (QUAD), FSL (DTIFIT), and FSL (BEDPOSTX), XTRACT

Susceptibility, EC, and HM

Whole brain; ROI

FA MD RD ODI NDI

Native space

23 tracts (including SLF, which seeds were central sulcus, SFG, ACG, MFG, and AnG)

NR

Positive correlation between phonological processing and the left IFOF, MDLF, SLF2, VOF, CBD and FX FA, and the l- UF MD

Huber E, et al. [32]

Phillips 3T

DKI

NR

NR

2

NR

0; 800; 2000

32 and 64

NR

FSL, DIPY, MRTrix, and AFQ

AP-PA, EC, Mean slice-by-slice displacement > 3 mm

Whole brain

FA MD AWF Da MDe

NR

AF, CS, UF, SLF, ILF, ThR, FMj, FMn, and IFOF

l-AF MD difference for Group x time interaction

Positive correlation between MD of l-AF, UF, l-ILF, l-IFOF, FMj, MDe of left of AF, UF, ILF, IFOF, and FMj with word reading and negative correlation between AWF of right ILF, IFOF, and FMn with word reading

Borghesani V, et al. [33]

Siemens 3T

DKI

8200/86

60

2.2

220x220

0; 700; 20,000

30 and 64

15 min

FSL (NODDI model), FS-TRACULA

AP-PA, EC, and HM

Voxel-based; ROI

NDI ODI

FSL, Desikan–Killiany Atlas and MNI

l-VOT

?ODI in DYS at the l-VOT

NR

Vander Stappen C, et al. [34]

Philips 3T

DTI

6422/83

70

2

224x224

800

55

NR

BrainVoyager

EC, HM

ROI

FA

Talairach space

AF, IFOF, and ILF

NR

RAN Gains negative correlated with FA in the l-long aAF, and the r-pAF, a reduction in naming times was linked to an increase in FA in those tracts at DYX

El-Sady S, et al. [35]

Philips 1.5T

DTI

NR

70

2

230x230

NR

32

NR

NR

EC, HM

ROI

FA ADC

NR

SLF, AF, CR, PLIC of CS

NR

Negative correlation between r-AF FA and at-risk quotient, l-sCR ADC with writing, and r-SLF ADC with bDS and positive with VF.Positive correlation between l-SLF-aCR FA and RAN, spelling, and VF, as r-PLIC ADC with writhing, and l-aCR ADC with bDS

Wang HLS, et al. [36]

Siemens 3T

DTI

6700/97

NR

2.7

NR

5000

128

NR

DSI Studio

NR

Whole brain

NR

MNI, AAL atlas

IFOF, CC, cerebellar, and Tha-pontine tracts

NR

l-IFOF, cerebellar, and Tha-pontine tracts had positive correlated with chinese character recognition; pCC association with auditory FM processing in DD

Vanderauwera J, et al, [37]

Phillips 3T

DTI

8872/2.5

55

2.5

240x240x137.5

1000

60

13:52 min

ExploreDTI, Trackvis

HM (>1.5 mm) and EC

ROI

FA

Native Space

AF, IFOF, UF, and ILF

NR

Word reading had positive correlation with l-long-AF FA and negative with l-long-AF RD and UF RD. Paternal educational level had positive correlation with l-long AF FA, and UF FA; after covariate by HM, only the l-UF remained significant

Lou C, et al. [38]

Siemens 3T

DTI

14,000/91

70

1.7

218

0; 1400

60

18 min (3x6 min)

ExploreDTI, FSL (FLIRT)

EC, HM

Whole brain

FA

AAL atlas; MNI; Harvard-Oxford atlas

Left of MTG-MOG, MOG-TPOsup, TPOsup-HG, HG-ROL, Ins-ROL, STG-Ins, and Ins-SMG

?mean FA in DYX for all ROIs

Literacy skills had positive correlation with clustering coefficient, local efficiency, transitivity, and global efficiency, in DYX

Lebel C, et al. [39]

Siemens 1.5T

SE EPI

9000/85

28

5

240x240

1000

NR

7:24 min

FSL

Motion artifacts (signal drop out, venetian blind artifact, and mechanical vibration artifact, >10), EC

ROI

FA MD AD RD

MNI; JHU ICBM- DTI-81 atlas

sCC, ALIC of CS, aCR, pCR, SS (includes the ILF and IFOF), UF, and SLF

?MD in r-CR, and l-UF in DYX

Age had a positive correlation with pCR, r-SLF FA, negative with pCR, l-UF MD. Sight words and VF were positively correlated with l-SLF FA and MD, respectively, as well as with l-pCR MD. Phonological decoding had a negative correlation with r-pCR MD and mean MD and positive with mean FA

Banfi C, et al. [40]

Siemens 3T

DTI

3400/105

48

2.5

240

0; 2000

64

NR

MRTrix, FSL, and AFQ

AP-PA, EC, HM, and susceptibility- induced distortion

Whole brain

FA

NR

ThR, FMj, FMn, IFOF, ILF, SLF, and AF, UF, CS, and CG

?FA in ILF, r-SLF, and r-CG in DYX

Negative correlation between r-ILF FA and reading measures, controlling for spelling.

Žaric G, et al. [41]

Siemens 3T

DTI

10,800/84(protocol1) 11,000/85(protocol2)

85

1.8

NR

0; 1000

72

15 min

VISTALab (mrDiffusion), SPM, AFQ

EC, HM and phase-encoding direction corrections

Whole brain; ROI

FA

NR

AF, SLF, ILF, IFOF, UF, lCS, antThR, FMn, and FMj

?FA in the AF, r-SLF, and aThR in DYX

r-SLF showed age effects that differed between groups. Age effect in ILF FA, and CC (FMj and FMn). L-aThR positive correlation with age appropriate reading accuracy scores

Su M, Zhao J, et al. [43]

Siemens 3T

DTI

8000/89

NR

2.2

282x282

0; 1000

30

NR (repeated twice)

ExploreDTI, Trackvis, and FSL

EC and HM

Whole brain; ROI

FA RD AD

MNI152; native space

AF, IFOF, and ILF

?FA and AD in the l-AF and I-ILF in DYX

AF and ILF FA positive correlation with character recognition, digit recall, phoneme deletion (only AF), and morphological production (only ILF). ILF FA negative correlation with RAN

Yagle K, et al. [42]

NR

DTI

8593/78

NR

2

220x220x128

0; 1000

32

9:35 min

FSL

NR

ROI

FA RA AD RD MD

NR

OR, CS, ILF, SLF, and CG

?FA in l-OR in DYX

NR

Christod- oulou JA, et al. [44]

Siemens 3T

DTI

9300/84

74

2

256

0; 700

30

NR

FS-TRACULA, DTIprep, and FSL (FLIRT)

EC, HM

Tract-based

FA AD RD

MNI152

SLF, AF

?FA in the l-AF in RDs

Positive correlation of l-AF FA and negative DA with real-word reading

Zhao JT, et al. [45]

Siemens 3T

DTI

14,000/91

70

1.7

218

0; 1400

60

18 min

ExploreDTI, FSL, and Trackvis

Motion corrections

Whole brain; ROI

HMOA FA

MNI152

IFOF, ILF, SLF, and AF

?FA of r-IFOF and l-SLF in DYX

r-IFOF FA negative correlation with reading and spelling accuracy

Koerte IK, et al. [46]

Siemens 3T

NR

9600/110

65

2

208

0; 1000

30

NR

3DSlicer, FSL (FLIRT), and FSL (TBSS)

EC, HM

Tract-based

FA AD RD trace

MNI152

NR

No group difference

Positive correlation arithmetic test with FA and AD and negative with RD (Temporo-parietal)

Garcia-Zapirain BG, et al. [47]

Philips 3T

DTI

6819/81

60

2

224x224

800

15

7min

FSL (BET), FSL (FDT), and FSL (TBSS)

NR

Whole-brain; ROI

FA MD AD RD

MNI; Atlas JHU White- matter

CC, SLF, ILF, lower FOF, l-AF, IFOF

?FA in l-AF in DYX

NR

Fernandez VG, et al. [48]

Philips 3T

DTI

6100/84

44

3

240x240

0; 1000

21

NR

FSL (DTIFIT)

EC, HM

ROI

FA AD RD

Desikan and Destrieux atlases

LAC/RAC to bilateral TP, OT, and IFG

?FA of cerebellar to TP and IFG; ?RD in TP in DYX

FA of AC-OT had interaction between age and group, younger DYX have ?FA in this region.

De Moura LM, et al. [49]

GE 1.5T

DTI

11,600/99

47

3

240x240

0; 800

15

NR

FSL, FSL(TBSS)

EC correction and non brain voxels removed

Voxel-based

FA RD MD AD

MNI152

aThR, CG, CS, IFOF, ILF, UF, FMj, FMn, and CGH

?FA left of aThR, CG, CS, FMj, FMn, UF, right of IFOF, ILF ?RD in the left of CG, CS, and SLF in DYX

NR

Richards TL, et al. [50]

Philips 3T

DTI

8593/78

NR

2.0

220x220x128

0; 1000

32

9 min 35 s

DTIPre (GTRAC), FSL (TBSS), and FSL

NR

ROI

FA AD RD RA MD

FSL white matter atlas (FHU)

aThR, FMn, CS, SLF, ILF, IFOF, UF, and CG

?RA in aThR, IFOF, SLF, UF, and l-CG, and FMn; ?AD in CS, r-ThR, CG, IFOF, SLF, and UF in DYX

NR

Marino C, et al. [51]

Philips 3T

DTI

9775/58

NR

2.3

NR

0; 1000

35

NR

BrainVoyager (Brainvisa), SPM

EC, smooth 6 mm

Voxel-based

FA

White matter atlases of FSL

ILF, IFOF, AF, SLF, CC, and OR

NR

DYX with DCDC2d gene x without found ?FA in ILF and l-CC

Fan Q, et al. [52]

Philips 3T

DTI

6237/75

60

2.2

212x212

0; 700

32

3 min 32 s

FSL (FDT), FS-TRACULA

EC, HM

ROI

FA

Desikan–Killiany Atlas

Thalamus to OFC, MPFC, LPFC, SMC, PC, MTC, LTC, OCC, and Ins

?FA of LPFC and SMC to ThR in DYX

Th-SMC showed negative correlation with basic reading score

Fan Q, et al. [53]

Philips 3T

DTI

6237/75

60

2.2

212x212

700

32

3 min 38s

FSL

EC, HM

ROI

NR

MNI152

5 ROIs of l-OT/F, MTG, ITG, LOCC, PaHipp, and ILF

Left Mid, Inf and sup- TG, lingual, fusiform, Sup and Inf PG in DYX

NR

Hasan KM, et al. [54]

Philips 3T

DTI

6100/84

44

3

NR

1000

21

min

NR

NR

ROI

FA MD AD RD Dav

NR

CC

?mFA of CC in DYX

MD and AD correlation with age (CC2); MD positive correlated with Letter- Word ID test in CC5

Gebauer D, et al. [55]

Siemens 3T

DTI

6700/95

35

2.5

250

NR

NR

NR

FSL (TBSS, FDT, DTIFIT, and BET)

EC

Voxel-based

FA

JHU ICBM-DTI-81 White-Matter Labels

aCR, CC

?FA in the l-aCR and aCC

NR

Hoeft F, et al. [56]

GE 3T

DTI

11,600/64.5

23

4

240

800

13

NR

SPM, DTIStudio, and ROQS

EC, HM

Whole-brain

FA

NR

SLF

NR

Positive correlation between r-SLF FA and single-word reading

Sihvonen AJ, et al. [57]

Siemens 3T

DTI

9000/80

70

2.5

240x240

0; 1000

64

NR

MRTrix, DSI Studio

Thermal noise with MP-PCA, Gibbs ringing correction

Whole brain

QA

MNI using (QSDR)

NR

?QA in VOF, SLF, AF, CC, CSl-UF, and ThR; ?QA in l-SLF, VOF, and CS in DYX

Reading skill positive association with l-CG and right fornix, and frontal corticopontine tracts and cerebellum

Tschentscher N, et al. [58]

Siemens 3T

DTI

12,900/100

88

1.7

220x220

0; 1000

60

6 min

FSL (FDT), FSL (PROBTRACKX), and FSL (BEDPOSTX)

Head motion corrections

Voxel-based; ROI

FA

MNI; Juelich histological; Harvard-Oxford atlases

A1, l-mPT, and MGB, IC

?connectivity between l-mPT-MGB in DYX

Negative correlation of l- mPT-MGB with reading skills in TR

Moreau D, et al. [59]

Siemens 1.5T

DTI

6601/101

NR

3

230

0; 1000

30

NR

FSL (DTIFIT), FSL (FLIRT), and FSL (TBSS)

EC and motion corrections

Whole brain; Voxel-based

FA

MNI152

Bilateral CR and AF

No group difference

NR

Müller-Axt C, et al. [60]

Siemens 3T

DTI

12,900/100

88

1.7

220x220

0; 1000

60

6 min

FSL

Motion correction

ROI

FA

Talairach; MNI; Juelich Histological atlas

LGN, l-V1, V5/MT

?LGN FA and between l-V5/MT-LGA in DYX

DYX showed negative correlation between l- V5/MT-LGN and name letters and numbers aloud time

Vanderm- osten M, et al. [61]

Philips 3T

DTI

11,043/55

68

2.2

220x220

0; 800

45

21 min 8 s

Explore DTI, FSL (CATNAP)

EC and motion- induced artifacts

Whole-brain; ROI

FA

Harvard-Oxford atlas in MNI space

Post STG, AF, sCC

NR

Positive correlation between coherence 20 Hz and FA of the STGp Lat and sCC in DYX and a negative in HC, without outliers

Lebel C, et al. [62]

Siemens 1.5T

DTI

9000/85

28

5

240x240

0; 1000

6

7 min 24 s

SPM

Smooth of 4 mm kernel

Voxel-based

FA MD

ICBM template

ALIC, sCC, ThR, CR, ILF, IFOF, anf aCR

NR

GORT fluency positive correleted with FA of aCC, sCC, right: aLimb, SLF, MCP, aCR, ILF, l-sCC, Th, IFOF; Word attack with FA of aCC, SLF, aLimb; l-Th, SLF, and r-IFOF

Vandermosten M, et al. [63]

Philips 3T

DTI

11,043/55

68

2.2

220x220

0; 800

45

21 min 8 s

Explore DTI,FSL (CATNAP)

EC, motion-induced artifacts correction

ROI

FA RD AD

Native space

AF, IFOF

?FA of l-AF in DYX

Direct and l-aAF FA positive correlated with phoneme awareness, and speech perception, respectively, and l-IFOF with orthography

Frye RE, et al. [64]

Philips 3T

DTI

6100/84

44

3

240x240

1000

NR

7 min

SPM

Distortion correction, masking, and isotropic voxel interpolation

Whole-brain

FA AD RD Dav

ICBM

FTP, SLF, SFOF, IFOF, and CR

No group difference

Negative correlated: FA- word attack in SLF, SFOF, aCR, and pCR; Dav- word attack in SLF; and positive correlation: Dav and AD—word attack in SFOF

Abbreviations: Ref.: Reference; MRI: magnetic resonance imaging; N: number; DTI: diffusion tensor image; DKI: diffusion kurtosis imaging; NR: Not reported; TR: Time of repetition; TE: time of echo; FOV: field of view; b: diffusion weighting; l: left; r: right; UF: Uncinate fasciculus; FAT: frontal aslant tract; EC: Eddy Current; ICBM: International Consortium for Brain Mapping; SLF: Superior longitudinal fasciculus; SFOF: superior frontal–occipital fasciculus.; IFOF: inferior frontal–occipital fasciculus; ROQS: Reproducible Objective Quantification Scheme; CC: corpus callosum; CC5: posterior midbody of CC; CC2: genu of CC; sCC: splenium of CC; aCC: anterior CC; VOT: ventral occipitotemporal cortex; OT/F: occipitotemporal/fusiform; SMC: supramarginal cortex; IFG: inferior frontal gyrus; FTP: frontal-temporo-parietal regions; TP: temporo-parietal regions; FMj: forceps major; FMn: forceps minor; ThR: thalamic radiations; aThR: anterior ThR; CG: cingulum; CS: corticalspinal tract; AF: arcuate fasciculus; aAF: anterior AF; pAF: posterior AF; ILF: inferior longitudinal fasciculus; HMOA: Hindrance-modulated oriented anisotropy; VAS: Visual Attention Span; AFQ: Automated Fiber Quantification software; SCP: superior cerebellar peduncle; ICP: inferior cerebellar peduncle; MCP: middle cerebellar peduncle; FA: fractional anisotropy, gFA: global white matter fractional anisotropy; AAL: automated anatomical labeling; MNI: Montreal Neurological Institute; SOG: superior occipital gyrus; MTG: middle temporal gyrus; ORBsupmed: medial orbital superior frontal gyrus; TR: typical readers; RD: reading disorder; QSDR: q-space diffeomorphic reconstruction; QA: quantitative anisotropy; FDT: FMRIB’s Diffusion Toolbox; MD: mean diffusivity; STG: superior temporal gyrus; ITG: inferior temporal gyrus; SMG: supramarginal gyrus; DYX: dyslexia; AnG: angular gyrus; IPL: inferior parietal lobe; IOG: inferior occipital gyrus; PreCG: precentral gyrus; ROL: Rolandic operculum; FFG: fusiform gyrus; QUAD: Quality Assessment of dMRI; DTIfit: diffusion tensor modeling tool; MDLF: middle longitudinal fasciculus; VOF: ventral occipital fasciculus; CBD: dorsal cingulum; FX: fornix; AWF: axonal water fraction; Da: intra-axonal diffusivity; MDe: extra-axonal mean diffusivity; NDI: neurite density index; ODI: orientation dispersion index; FHD+: positive familial risk to develop dyslexia; FHD-: negative familial risk to develop dyslexia; CR: corona radiata; aCR: anterior CR; pCR: posterior CR; PLIC: posterior limb of internal capsule; FM: frequency modulation; A1: primary auditory cortex; mPT: planum temporale; MGB: medial geniculate body; IC: inferior colliculus; BEDPOSTX: Bayesian Estimation of Diffusion Parameters Obtained using Sampling Techniques; MOG: middle occipital gyrus; -TPOsup: temporal pole; HG: Heschl’s gyrus; AD: axial diffusivity; RD: radial diffusivity; ALIC: anterior limb of the internal capsule; SD: spelling disorder; TBSS: Tract-based spatial statistics; FLIRT: FMRIB linear image registration tool; RA: relative anisotropy; SLD: specific learning disability; WM: white matter; TP: temporoparietal; LGN: lateral geniculate nucleus; V1: primary visual cortex; V5/MT: middle temporal area; TRACULA: TRActs Constrained by UnderLying Anatomy; TP-AF: temporo-paietal portion of the AF; ASD: autism spectrum disorder; SOS/TOS: superior and transversal occipital sulci; BET: Brain Extraction Tool; LAC: left anterior cerebellum; RAC: right anterior cerebellum; HC: health control; JHU: Johns Hopkins School; OFC: orbitofrontal cortex; MPFC: medial prefrontal cortex; LPFC: lateral prefrontal cortex; PC: parietal cortex; MTC: medial temporal cortex; LTC: lateral temporal cortex; OCC: occipital cortex; Ins: insular cortex; LOCC: lateral OCC; PaHipp: parahippocampal regions; GORT: Gray Oral Reading Test; aLimb: anterior limb; SS: sagital stratum; IC: inferior colliculus; OR: Opptic radiation; IFoG: pars opercularis of inferior frontal gyrus; IFtG: pars triangularis of IFG; IFobG: pars orbitalis of IFG; bDS: backward digit span; and VF: verbal fluency.

Author Affiliation(s):

[1] Laboratório de Investigação Médica em Neurorradiologia—LIM44—Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo 05403-000, Brazil; [emailprotected] (B.M.); [emailprotected] (M.Y.B.); [emailprotected] (E.M.D.)

[2] Centro de Matemática, Computação e Cognição (CMCC), Universidade Federal do ABC, Santo André 09210-580, Brazil; [emailprotected] (L.F.C.); [emailprotected] (A.S.C.); [emailprotected] (K.L.)

Author Note(s):

[*] Correspondence: [emailprotected]; Tel.: +55-11-2661-7916

DOI: 10.3390/brainsci14040349

COPYRIGHT 2024 MDPI AG
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.

Copyright 2024 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.


Investigating Dyslexia through Diffusion Tensor Imaging across Ages: A Systematic Review. (2024)

References

Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Greg Kuvalis

Last Updated:

Views: 6332

Rating: 4.4 / 5 (55 voted)

Reviews: 94% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Greg Kuvalis

Birthday: 1996-12-20

Address: 53157 Trantow Inlet, Townemouth, FL 92564-0267

Phone: +68218650356656

Job: IT Representative

Hobby: Knitting, Amateur radio, Skiing, Running, Mountain biking, Slacklining, Electronics

Introduction: My name is Greg Kuvalis, I am a witty, spotless, beautiful, charming, delightful, thankful, beautiful person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.